Skip to main content

Verbal labels increase the salience of novel objects for preschoolers with typical development and Williams syndrome, but not in autism

Abstract

Background

Early research has documented that young children show an increased interest toward objects that are verbally labeled by an adult, compared to objects that are presented without a label. It is unclear whether the same phenomenon occurs in neurodevelopmental disorders affecting social development, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and Williams syndrome (WS).

Methods

The present study used a novel eye-tracking paradigm to determine whether hearing a verbal label increases the salience of novel objects in 35 preschoolers with ASD, 18 preschoolers with WS, and 20 typically developing peers.

Results

We found that typically developing children and those with WS, but not those with ASD, spent significantly more time looking at objects that are verbally labeled by an adult, compared to objects that are presented without a label.

Conclusions

In children without ASD, information accompanied by the speaker’s verbal label is accorded a “special status,” and it is more likely to be attended to. In contrast, children with ASD do not appear to attribute a special salience to labeled objects compared to non-labeled objects. This result is consistent with the notion that reduced responsivity to pedagogical cues hinders social learning in young children with ASD.

Background

Human learning is a selective process [13]. From infancy onward, children are more likely to pay attention to and acquire knowledge from stimuli that are accompanied by another’s communicative (“pedagogical”) cues, such as eye contact and infant-directed speech and body language [46]. For example, typically developing preschoolers are more likely to focus on and imitate a novel action if the model establishes mutual gaze before the demonstration [7, 8] or acts in a socially engaging manner [9, 10].

Another cue that enhances children’s attention to specific information in the environment is the presence of a verbal label. Baldwin and colleagues [11, 12] were the first to demonstrate that infants spend significantly more time looking at and manually exploring objects that are verbally labeled by an adult, compared to objects that are presented without a label. Several theoretical perspectives emphasize this as a critical process that facilitates language acquisition (see [1315]).

The socially guided selection of relevant “to-be-learned” information reflects an early emerging sensitivity to the pedagogical structure underlying social learning. This process involves a knowledgeable adult guiding the child’s attention to relevant features in the environment through various ostensive cues, and a child equipped with dedicated cognitive resources to notice, read, and take advantage of such cues [1618]. A corollary of this model is that children who are more socially attuned may be better equipped to access and use pedagogical information to learn from relevant aspects of the social environment.

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and Williams syndrome (WS) provide model disorders to investigate this notion. From infancy, children with ASD and those with WS share some overlapping deficits in the use of gesture, pointing, joint attention, and social reasoning [1922]. However, they present with opposing profiles in their propensity to attend to and engage with their social environment, which is atypically low in ASD [23] and atypically high in WS [24]. It remains unclear how these distinct profiles of social engagement affect children’s ability to decipher and take advantage of pedagogical cues in order to select relevant “to-be-learned” information in the environment.

Children with ASD show a reduced propensity and ability to attend to and interpret social information, which impacts their ability to learn from others [2527]. Unlike typically developing children and peers with other developmental disorders, children with ASD fail to increase their attention and spontaneous imitation in response to actions that are accompanied by eye contact [7], infant-directed body language [28], and referential looks to relevant stimuli [2932]. Additionally, evidence suggests that children with ASD fail to use social cues such as the speaker’s referential gaze for word learning [3335], although counter-evidence exists [36]. A recent study using eye-tracking showed that the presence of verbal labels affects attention toward novel objects in some (but not all) children with ASD [37]. Similarly, using a behavioral paradigm, McDuffie et al. [38] reported that preschoolers with ASD, just like their typically developing peers, increase their interest toward objects that are verbally labeled by an adult speaker—suggesting intact sensitivity to this pedagogical cue. However, another study [39] reported failure to increase attention to novel objects in response to verbal labels in both children with ASD and typically developing preschoolers. Conflicting results in this area of research might reflect methodological differences. In particular, the McDuffie et al. study used various manipulations, such as the use of infant-directed speech and body motion, aimed at increasing the attention of children with ASD, while other studies (e.g., [7, 32]) were designed to capture spontaneous attention and learning in situations where adults did not attempt to facilitate or enhance the child’s performance. These contrasting findings point to the need for additional research to determine the extent to which children with ASD are able to register and take advantage of different types of pedagogical cues.

Less is known about sensitivity to pedagogical cues in individuals with Williams syndrome (WS), a rare neurodevelopmental disorder (estimated prevalence of 1:7500 to 1:20,000; [40]) characterized by impaired visual-spatial abilities and social-pragmatic skills alongside an increased drive for social approach [41]. While several studies have documented increased attention and interest toward social stimuli in WS, especially toward faces and eyes [21, 42], as well as an enhanced imitation of facial expressions of emotions [43], some authors have reported diminished ability to “read” the meaning of people’s gaze and facial expressions [44, 45] and difficulties in the social-pragmatic skills needed to navigate the social environment effectively [4648]. These findings leave open the question of how the unique social phenotype in WS affects the ability to make use of pedagogical cues to guide social learning.

The current study aimed to investigate whether verbal labels increase the salience of novel objects in young children with ASD and WS. To this end, we used a novel eye-tracking paradigm adapted from Baldwin et al.’s seminal experiment on verbal labeling [11]. We examined the extent to which preschoolers with ASD, WS, and typical development increase their visual attention to, and manual exploration of, objects that are accompanied by a verbal label relative to non-labeled objects. This paradigm afforded an exploration of the attention-facilitating effect of verbal labels in ASD, in typical development, and, for the first time, in preschoolers with WS, by taking advantage of the measurement precision provided by eye-tracking technology. Eye-tracking approaches are considered to be optimally suited for research on children with neurodevelopmental disorders who experience difficulties following verbal instructions or are distressed by social demands, such as ASD and WS [49].

Our hypothesis was that children with ASD, unlike those with WS and typically developing children, would not show more interest toward novel objects that are verbally labeled by an adult compared to objects that are presented without a verbal label. Therefore, we predicted that typically developing children and those with WS would be more inclined to pay attention to and play with objects that were labeled by an adult versus objects that were not labeled. Conversely, we predicted that propensity to pay attention to and play with objects in children with ASD would be unaffected by the presence of a verbal label. Finally, we expected a similar pattern of associations across groups, with children who are more responsive to verbal labels showing better cognitive and language skills.

Methods

Participants

The participants were 35 preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), 18 preschoolers with William syndrome (WS), and 20 typically developing children (TD) who were matched on chronological age and, in the case of the ASD and WS groups, cognitive and language level. The children with ASD were recruited through the Victorian Autism Specific Early Learning and Care Centre, an intervention program located at the La Trobe University Community Children’s Centre. Participants in the WS group were recruited through the Williams Syndrome Family Support Group (Victoria) and the Williams Syndrome Association Australia, and those in the TD group were recruited through a childcare service located in the Macquarie University Campus. Parents provided informed consent after reading a participant information sheet about the study and ethics approval for the consent procedure in this study was provided by the La Trobe Human Ethics Committee (reference no. 14-007).

Diagnoses of ASD were previously made by community-based health care professionals and confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS 2, [50]) administered by a clinician with demonstrated reliability in the use of this measure. Exclusion criteria included the presence of uncorrected hearing or vision impairment and the presence of major medical problems. All participants with WS had their diagnosis confirmed with the positive fluorescent in situ hybridization (FISH) test and displayed the typical ~1.6 Mb heterozygous microdeletion at 7q11.23 [51].

Participants’ cognitive level was measured with the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; [52]), and their adaptive behavior was assessed using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS; [53]). Developmental quotient (DQ) scores were calculated according to the formula: DQ = age equivalent scores/chronological age × 100, and averaged to create an overall DQ, a verbal DQ (encompassing the receptive and expressive language subscales), and a non-verbal DQ (encompassing the visual reception and fine motor subscales). The ASD and WS groups did not differ on language or cognitive level, motor skills, and overall adaptive behavior (Table 1). However, as expected, children with WS had higher scores on the socialization subscale of the VABS compared to the children with ASD. Both clinical groups had significantly lower scores on each measure compared to children in the TD group.

Table 1 Participants’ characteristics

Procedure

Participants were tested in a quiet room in one of three University or early intervention settings, depending on where the child was recruited. Three children were tested in their home. The length of experimental testing was approximately 20 min. The experiments presented here were part of a larger study.

Using a procedure inspired by Baldwin and Markman [11], we investigated whether hearing a verbal label increases the salience of novel objects through two experimental phases. First, we investigated whether participants increased their visual attention to an object that was verbally labeled by a speaker compared to objects that were accompanied by a non-verbal ostensive cue, i.e., pointing. Second, during a free play period involving the same objects that were presented previously, we measured duration of time that children manually explored the labeled and non-labeled objects.

Phase 1

Children were shown a series of four video stimuli (10 s each) on a Tobii T120 binocular eye-tracker monitor with an embedded camera (120-Hz, 1280 × 1024 pixel resolution, average precision of 0.5 of visual angle). As illustrated in Fig. 1, in each video, a female actor had a different set of four objects placed on the table in front of her. She first looked up ostensibly at the child and then pointed to each of the objects. The duration of each pointing episode was 2 s. For one object (the “labeled object”) in each video, pointing was also accompanied by a verbal label. The actor displayed neutral affect throughout the video. The 16 objects were chosen so that young children would be unfamiliar with their labels (e.g., “floppy disk,” “hole punch,” “protractor”). The position of the labeled object was different in each of the four trials. At the end of each video, the four objects were shown on a still-frame image for 4 s, which had no audio. The presentation of the video-stimuli was arranged in two fixed random orders, which were counterbalanced across participants. Videos were interspersed with filler stimuli to maintain attention.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Example of video-stimuli (trial 1). The actor points to each objects, adding the verbal label when pointing to the third object. b Sets of objects used in the four trials

Participants were seated in a comfortable chair, 60 cm from the computer monitor in front of a small table, with no explicit instructions given. Their attention in response to each pointing and verbal labeling episode and each pointing only episode during observation of the videos was measured to determine whether the labeled objects were attended more compared with the non-labeled objects. To this end, participants’ eye-movements were recorded using the eye-tracker system and analyzed using frame-by-frame defined areas of interest using Tobii Studio analysis software. Fixation criteria were set to Tobii Studio defaults of a 30-pixel dispersion threshold for 100 ms. The regions of interest included in the analyses were the labeled object, the non-labeled objects, and the actor’s face. Four participants in the ASD group and one in the WS group were excluded due to either a calibration error or equipment failure during the experiment.

Phase 2

Immediately following phase 1, participants were invited to sit on a mat and were presented with the same labeled and non-labeled objects that were previously presented in the four videos. All the objects used across the four trials were presented at the same time. The experimenter encouraged the child to play with the objects, but no specific instructions were given, and there was no active interaction with the child during this free play. In particular, the experimenter did not label any object during this phase. The duration of the free play session was 180 s. Participants’ spontaneous behavior with the objects was video-recorded, and duration of manual exploration of each object (operationalized as any episode during which children had an object in their hand for at least 1 s) was later coded by a research assistant blind to diagnostic group and study hypotheses.

Results

Phase 1

Preliminary analyses indicated that participants’ duration of attention to the objects shown in the videos was unaffected by the setting in which the experiment took place (for labeled objects F = 1.7, p = 0.2; for non-labeled objects F = 0.15, p = 0.9). Additionally, in order to rule out the possibility that the labeled-object was more visually salient than the other objects at baseline, we analyzed participants’ total duration of attention to the objects during the period of time before the actor began pointing/labeling the objects (1.5 s). This analysis showed that the labeled objects were not attended to longer than the other objects across the groups. As illustrated in Fig. 2, attention to the target object and the non-labeled objects before the actor began pointing was similar in the ASD group (p = 0.83) and in the WS group (p = 0.21), while in the TD group, there was more attention in response to non-labeled object compared to the target object (p < 0.05), suggesting that the labeled object was not more visually salient compared to the other objects.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean fixation duration to the labeled and non-labeled objects in ASD, WS, and TD groups before the objects are labeled. Y-axis represents seconds

Next, participants’ visual attention to the labeled and non-labeled objects (attention duration) was subjected to a 3 (Group) × 2 (Condition-labeled, non-labeled objects) repeated measures ANOVA. Chronological age was included as a covariate term. There was no main effect of Condition (F (2, 64) = 1.15, p = 0.69, η 2 p  = 0.00), a main effect of Group (F (2, 64) = 4.26, p = 0.01, η 2 p  = 0.11), and a significant Group × Condition interaction (F (2, 64) = 5.28, p < 0.01, η 2 p  = 0.14). There was no effect of covariate chronological age, (F (2, 64) = 0.07, p = .80, η 2 p  = 0.00). As illustrated in Fig. 3, pairwise comparisons showed that while participants in the WS and TD groups looked longer to the labeled compared to the non-labeled objects (in WS adjusted p [Bonferroni] < 0.05, η 2 p  = .06, in TD adjusted p [Bonferroni] < 0.01, η2 p = 0.11), this was not the case in the ASD group (adjusted p = 0.19, η 2 p  = .02).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean fixation duration to the labeled and non-labeled objects in ASD, WS, and TD groups after the objects are labeled. Y-axis represents seconds. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01

We then analyzed how participants changed their attention from the non-labeled objects prior to labeling episode to the labeled object after the object was labeled. To this aim, we used a 3 (Group) × 2 (Condition-mean fixation duration to the non-labeled objects prior to the labeling episode, mean fixation duration to the labeled object after the labeling episode) repeated measures ANOVA. Chronological age was included as a covariate term. There was an effect of Condition (F (2, 64) = 22.74, p < 0.001, η 2 p  = 0.27), a main effect of Group (F (2, 64) = 4.29, p = 0.01, η 2 p  = 0.12), and a significant Group × Condition interaction (F (2, 64) = 4.29, p = 0.01, η 2 p  = 0.12). There was no effect of covariate chronological age, (F (2, 64) = 2.10, p = 0.15, η 2 p  = 0.03). Pairwise comparisons showed that before the labeling episodes, attention to the objects was similar in the ASD group compared to the WS group (adjusted p [Bonferroni] = 1.00) and to the TD group (adjusted p [Bonferroni] = 0.18). Similarly, there was no difference between the TD and the WS group (adjusted p [Bonferroni] = 0.31). However, attention to the target object after the object was labeled was greater in the TD group compared to the ASD group (adjusted p [Bonferroni] < 0.05). Additionally, the TD group showed a trend in the same direction when compared to the WS group (adjusted p [Bonferroni] = 0.09). There was no significant difference between the ASD and WS group in this condition (adjusted p [Bonferroni] = 1.00).

Finally, participants’ visual attention to the face of the adult across conditions was subjected to a 3 (Group) × 2 (Condition-labeling, non-labeling condition) repeated measures ANOVA. Chronological age was included as a covariate term. There was no main effect of Condition (F (2, 64) = 0.26, p = 0.61, η 2 p  = 0.00), Group (F (2, 64) = 1.88, p = 0.16, η 2 p  = 0.05), Group × Condition interaction (F (2, 64) = 2.15, p = 0.12, η 2 p  = .06), or covariate chronological age (F (2, 64) = 0.14, p = 0.70, η 2 p  = 0.00).

Phase 2

In order to determine whether participants explored the labeled objects more than non-labeled objects, we calculated participants’ average duration of manual exploration (number of seconds spent touching the object) of the labeled and non-labeled objects during the free play session. We then conducted a 3 (Group) × 2 (Condition-labeled objects, non-labeled objects) ANOVA. Chronological age was included as a covariate term. There was no main effect of Condition (F (2, 62) = 2.66, p = 0.11, η 2 p  = 0.04), Group (F (2, 62) = 0.59, p = .55, η 2 p  = 0.01), Group × Condition interaction (F (2, 62) = 0.59, p = 0.55 η2 p  = 0.01), or covariate chronological age (F (2, 62) = 3.0, p = 0.09, η 2 p  = 0.04).

Pearson correlations between visual attention and duration of manual exploration of labeled objects and measures of cognitive, social, and language abilities were examined in each group. In the ASD group, attention to the labeled objects was positively associated with verbal DQ (r = 0.45, p = 0.01) as well as Vineland Communication standard score (r = 0.39, p < 0.05) and negatively associated with severity of social symptoms (ADOS Social Affect score, r = −0.57, p = 0.001). In the WS group, there was a positive association between attention to the labeled objects and verbal DQ (r = 0.57, p = 0.01). There were no other significant associations between attention and exploration of labeled objects and any other participant characteristics.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated whether children with ASD and WS, two neurodevelopmental disorders presenting with different patterns of atypical social attention and social learning, allocate more attentional resources to objects that are verbally labeled by a speaker compared to objects that are not labeled. To this end, we examined spontaneous visual attention and manual exploration of novel objects under conditions that varied only in the presence or absence of a verbal label. Our findings showed that hearing a label increases visual attention to unfamiliar objects in typically developing preschoolers and those with WS, but not those with ASD. This is consistent with the notion that adding a verbal label to other non-verbal indicating behaviors has an attention-facilitating effect on typical development, thus providing a pedagogical cue to guide selection of relevant information in the environment [11, 12, 54]. Additionally, these results extend our knowledge on this process in developmental disorders affecting social engagement, indicating that sensitivity and responsiveness to verbal labels is relatively preserved in WS but not in children with ASD.

Interestingly, however, these results contrast with those from a recent study by Benjamin et al. [39] who reported that typically developing preschoolers (mean age 3.5 years), as well as school-aged children with ASD (mean age 7 years), failed to increase their attention to novel objects in response to verbal labels. In the Benjamin et al. study, the labeling condition was contrasted to a condition where a speaker was talking (without labeling) about the target objects using child-directed and playful language and motion. It is possible that these cues, which were not used in the current study, caused children to pay the same amount of attention across conditions. Moreover, the children with ASD in their study were significantly older than the comparison group.

Similarly, our results contrast with those reported by McDuffie et al. [38], who documented a similar attention facilitating effect of verbal labeling in preschoolers with ASD and typical development. Importantly, methodological differences might account for the different findings. Like in the Benjamin et al. study, the non-labeling condition in the McDuffie et al. study involved a series of manipulations aimed at increasing the attention of children with ASD, including infant-directed speech (without labels) and body motion. In the current study, we opted to use a procedure inspired by the original Baldwin and Markman [11] study, which involved the use of an “emotionally neutral” adult labeling and pointing to the objects. This allowed us to evaluate whether it is verbal labeling alone which enhances children’s attention, without the potentially confounding factor of playfulness/child-directed speech.

Interestingly, however, we did not replicate one of the original findings from the Baldwin and Markman [11] study, which showed that typically developing children play for longer with labeled versus non-labeled objects. Rather, we found that participants across groups played with the objects for a similar duration of time across conditions. This finding may reflect the differences in age of participants between our studies, where Baldwin and Markman focused on infants between 10 and 20 months of age, whereas the current study examined verbal labeling in older preschoolers with typical and atypical development. Another possible explanation for this discrepancy is that one aspect of the procedure in the current study differed from the Baldwin and Markman study; in the original paradigm, labeled and non-labeled objects were presented in pairs, and the non-labeled object was not pointed to as was the case in the current study.

Importantly, our study documents for the first time that exposure to a verbal label also has a facilitative effect on attention to labeled objects in preschoolers with WS. This finding appears to be in contrast to previous reports indicating difficulties in using social-communicative cues to guide learning in children with WS. For example, a number of studies have reported difficulties in following the eye gaze of interlocutors, reduced attention to the referential targets of pointing, and reduced ability to use linguistic labels to categorize objects, as well as weaknesses in reading communicative intent in pointing, gestures, and eye gaze [5557]. These difficulties in early social learning processes have been linked to social-communication difficulties in children with WS from infancy onward, despite their fascination for social stimuli [45, 58]. In our study, however, while communication skills (as well as cognitive abilities) in the WS group were significantly delayed and comparable to those of the ASD group, the young children with WS did not differ from age-matched typically developing children in their ability to attribute salience to verbally labeled objects. This process might be less cognitively demanding compared to the ability to read communicative facial and gestural cues. It is possible that this intact sensitivity to verbal labels facilitates language development in this population, despite the presence of cognitive and social difficulties. Further research based on longitudinal designs is needed to substantiate these speculations.

In contrast, our findings showed that preschoolers with ASD did not allocate more attentional resources to labeled versus non-labeled objects. This finding might be interpreted in the context of a diminished sensitivity to the pedagogical structure of social learning in this population [25]. Previous studies have documented that children with ASD show a diminished tendency to register, prioritize, and take advantage of the learning opportunities conveyed by cues such as pointing [59], eye contact [7], referential use of gaze [32], infant-directed speech [60], child-directed non-verbal communication [28], hearing a speaker calling one’s own name [61], and the presence of verbal labels [37]. These early emerging abnormalities have been linked to atypical language development in this population [62]. Importantly, by contrasting responses to a “pointing only” condition versus a “pointing plus labeling” condition, our findings show that deficits in ASD extend beyond difficulties in response to joint attention and involve a reduced responsivity to the “added value” of verbal labeling as an attention facilitating cue.

We found significant associations between attention to the labeled objects and measures of language and social communication in ASD. Similarly, children with WS who paid longer attention to the labeled objects had better communication skills. This finding is consistent with the notion of a link between responsiveness to the pedagogical value of verbal labels and language development and is relevant to the debate on whether language difficulties in ASD reflect specific challenges with social learning of words versus general word learning [37, 63, 64]. However, the correlational nature of the link between attentional patterns in response to labels and language proficiency documented in the current study does not provide causal information regarding the directionality of this association. Longitudinal research is now needed to study the development of the use of pedagogical cues and language development over time in typical and atypical development.

There are some limitations in the current study that should be acknowledged. One possible limitation concerns the use of video-recorded stimuli which, while allowing for rigorous eye-tracking measurement of visual attention, might be qualitatively different from the social exchanges that support processing of pedagogical cues in real life. Additionally, consistent with reports of distractibility/inattention in both children with ASD and those with WS [65, 66], the main effect of Group in the ANOVA measuring visual attention indicated that overall children in the TD groups were paying more attention to the videos compared to both the ASD and WS groups. However, there was no difference in overall attention between children with ASD and those with WS, therefore ruling out domain-general attention difficulties as an alternative explanation for why children with WS but not those with ASD increased their attention to the labeled versus non-labeled objects. An additional limitation involves the lack of inter-rater reliability procedures for the coding of manual exploration duration. Finally, the ASD group showed the gender imbalance that characterizes the ASD population, involving many more boys than girls, whereas the WS and TD groups were more gender balanced. It cannot be excluded that some of the group differences reported here are due to the fact that language development in boys progresses more slowly than in girls.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the current study provides the first direct comparison of the facilitative effect of verbal labels in preschoolers with typical development, ASD, and WS using an eye-tracking paradigm. This methodology likely offers more precision in capturing attentional increase (or lack thereof) in response to pedagogical cues compared to behavioral coding techniques and is optimally suited for research involving children with communication difficulties such as those with ASD and WS [49]. We found that in children without ASD, information accompanied by the speaker’s verbal label is accorded a “special status,” and it is more likely to be attended to. In contrast, children with ASD do not appear to attribute a special salience to labeled objects compared to non-labeled objects.

These findings provide support to the notion that preschoolers are more likely to attend to features of the environment that are “signaled” by an adult’s pedagogical cues, a phenomenon considered to be crucial for cultural transmission, knowledge acquisition, and social development [4, 67]. Conversely, reduced responsivity to relevant communicative cues might hinder social learning in young children with ASD [25, 68]. This latter result points to the need for targeted intervention strategies that facilitate processing of pedagogical cues during early social exchanges.

Abbreviations

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

WS:

Williams syndrome

ADOS:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

MSEL:

Mullen Scales of Early Learning

VABS:

Vineland Scales of Adaptive Behavior

References

  1. Miller NE, Dollard J. Social learning and imitation. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1941.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Pagel M. Wired for culture: The natural history of human cooperation. London: Penguin; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Koenig MA, Sabbagh MA. Selective social learning: new perspectives on learning from others. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(3):399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Gergely G, Csibra G. Sylvia’s recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In: Levenson S, Enfield N, editors. Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition, and Human Interaction (pp. 229–255). Oxford: Berg Publishers; 2006.

  5. Frith CD. Social cognition. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363(1499):2033–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Sage KD, Baldwin D. Disentangling the social and the pedagogical in infants’ learning about tool use. Soc Dev. 2011;20(4):825–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Vivanti G, Dissanayake C. Propensity to imitate in autism is not modulated by the model’s gaze direction: an eye tracking study. Autism Res. 2014;7(3):392–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Wang Y, Newport R, Hamilton AF. Eye contact enhances mimicry of intransitive hand movements. Biol Lett. 2011;7(1):7–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Williamson RA, Brand RJ. Child-directed action promotes 2-year-olds’ imitation. J Exp Child Psychol. 2014;118:119–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Nielsen M, Simcock G, Jenkins L. The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds’ imitation from live and televised models. Dev Sci. 2008;11(5):722–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Baldwin DA, Markman EM. Establishing word-object relations: a first step. Child development. 1989;60(2):381.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Baldwin DA, Moses LJ. Links between social understanding and early word learning: challenges to current accounts. Soc Dev. 2001;10(3):309–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Bloom P. Mindreading, communication and the learning of names for things. Mind Lang. 2002;17(1‐2):37–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Tan S, Schafer G. Toddlers’ novel word learning: effects of phonological representation, vocabulary size, and parents’ ostensive behaviour. First Lang. 2005;25:131–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Colombo J, McCardle P, Freund L. Infant pathways to language: methods, models, and research directions. New York: Psychology Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Gergely G. Ostensive communication and cultural learning: The natural pedagogy hypothesis. In: Metcalfe J, Terrace HS, editors. Agency and Joint Attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 139.

  17. Wu R, Tummeltshammer KS, Gliga T, Kirkham NZ. Ostensive signals support learning from novel attention cues during infancy. Front Psychol. 2014;25;5:251.

  18. Leekam SR, Solomon TL, Teoh YS. Adults’ social cues facilitate young children’s use of signs and symbols. Dev Sci. 2010;13(1):108–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Klein-Tasman BP, Mervis CB, Lord C, Phillips KD. Socio communicative deficits in young children with Williams syndrome: performance on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Child Neuropsychol. 2007;13(5):444–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Lincoln AJ, Searcy YM, Jones W, Lord C. Social interaction behaviors discriminate young children with autism and Williams syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(3):323–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Riby DM, Hancock PJ, Jones N, Hanley M. Spontaneous and cued gaze-following in autism and Williams syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2013;5(1):13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Hanley M, Riby DM, Caswell S, Rooney S, Back E. Looking and thinking: how individuals with Williams syndrome make judgements about mental states. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(12):4466–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Dawson G, Bernier R. A quarter century of progress on the early detection and treatment of autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(4pt2):1455–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Karmiloff-Smith A, Broadbent H, Farran EK, Longhi E, D’Souza D, Metcalfe K, Tassabehji M, Wu R, Senju A, Happé F, Turnpenny P. Social cognition in Williams syndrome: genotype/phenotype insights from partial deletion patients. Front Psychol. 2012;3:168.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Vivanti G, Rogers SJ. Autism and the mirror neuron system: insights from learning and teaching. Philos Trans R Soc London. 2014;369:20130184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Jones W, Klin A. Social engagement in the first two years of life in Autism Spectrum Disorders. In: Just MA, Pelphrey KA, editors. Development and brain systems in autism. New York: Psychology Press; 2013. p. 123–47.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Dawson G, Sterling L, Faja S. Autism: risk factors, risk processes, and outcome. In: de Haan M, Gunnar M, eds. Handbook of Developmental Social Neuroscience. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. p. 435–58.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Vivanti G, Hocking DR, Fanning P, Dissanayake C. Social affiliation motives modulate spontaneous learning in Williams syndrome but not in autism. Mol Autism. 2016;7(1):40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Jolliffe AT. Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Vis Cogn. 1997;4(3):311–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Gliga T, Elsabbagh M, Hudry K, Charman T, Johnson MH. Gaze following, gaze reading, and word learning in children at risk for autism. Child Dev. 2012;83(3):926–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Preissler MA, Carey S. The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: evidence from autism. Cognition. 2005;97(1):B13–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Vivanti G, McCormick C, Young GS, Abucayan F, Hatt N, Nadig A, Rogers SJ. Intact and impaired mechanisms of action understanding in autism. Dev Psychol. 2011;47(3):841.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Naigles LR, Chin I. Language development in children with autism. In: Bavin E, Naigles L, editors. Cambridge handbook of child language, 2nd. Cambridge: CUP; 2015. p. 637–58.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  34. Parish‐Morris J, Hennon EA, Hirsh‐Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, Tager‐Flusberg H. Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning. Child Dev. 2007;78(4):1265–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Baron-Cohen S, Baldwin DA, Crowson M. Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Dev. 1997;68(1):48–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Hani HB, Gonzalez-Barrero AM, Nadig AS. Children’s referential understanding of novel words and parent labeling behaviors: similarities across children with and without autism spectrum disorders. J Child Lang. 2013;40(05):971–1002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Potrzeba ER, Fein D, Naigles L. Investigating the shape bias in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorders. Front Psychol. 2014;6:446.

    Google Scholar 

  38. McDuffie AS, Yoder PJ, Stone WL. Labels increase attention to novel objects in children with autism and comprehension-matched children with typical development. Autism. 2006;10(3):288–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Benjamin DP, McDuffie AS, Thurman AJ, Kover ST, Mastergeorge AM, Hagerman RJ, Abbeduto L. Effect of speaker gaze on word learning in fragile X syndrome: a comparison with nonsyndromic autism spectrum disorder. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2015;58(2):383–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. StrØmme P, BjØrnstad PG, Ramstad K. Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. J Child Neurol. 2002;17:269–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Hocking D. Williams syndrome. In: Rinehart N, Bradshaw JL, Enticott P, editors. Developmental Disorders of the Brain; In press.

  42. Riby DM, Hancock P. Do faces capture the attention of individuals with Williams syndrome or autism? Evidence from tracking eye movements. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(3):421–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Fidler DJ, Hepburn SL, Most DE, Philofsky A, Rogers SJ. Emotional responsivity in young children with Williams syndrome. Am J Ment Retard. 2007;112(3):194-206.

  44. Porter MA, Coltheart M, Langdon R. The neuropsychological basis of hypersociability in Williams and Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(12):2839–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Tager-Flusberg H, Skwerer D. Social engagement does not lead to social cognition: evidence from Williams syndrome. In: Banaji M, Gelman S, editors. Navigating the social world. New York: Oxford Press; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Plesa Skwerer D, Tager‐Flusberg H. Empathic responsiveness and helping behaviours in young children with Williams syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2016;60:1010–9. doi:10.1111/jir.12302.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Plesa Skwerer D, Ammerman E, Tager-Flusberg H. Do you have a question for me? How children with Williams syndrome respond to ambiguous referential communication during a joint activity. J Child Lang. 2013;40(01):266–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Sparaci L, Stefanini S, D’Elia L, Vicari S, Rizzolatti G. What and why understanding in autism spectrum disorders and Williams syndrome: similarities and differences. Autism Res. 2014;7(4):421–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Venker CE, Kover ST. An open conversation on using eye-gaze methods in studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2015;58(6):1719–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–2nd edition (ADOS-2). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Corporation. 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Porter M, Dobson-Stone C, Kwok J, Schofield P, Beckett W, Tassabehji M. A role for transcription factor GTF2IRD2 in executive function in Williams-Beuren syndrome. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e47457.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Mullen EM. Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Sparrow S, Balla D, Cicchetti D. Vineland adaptive behavior scales. 2nd ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Flom R, Pick AD. Verbal encouragement and joint attention in 18-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2003;26(2):121–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Laing E, Butterworth G, Ansari D, Gsödl M, Longhi E, Panagiotaki G, Karmiloff‐Smith A. Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Dev Sci. 2002;5(2):233–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Nazzi T, Gopnik A, Karmiloff-smith A. Asynchrony in the cognitive and lexical development of young children with Williams syndrome. J Child Lang. 2005;32(02):427–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. John AE, Mervis CB. Comprehension of the communicative intent behind pointing and gazing gestures by young children with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53:950–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Hsu CF, Karmiloff-Smith A. Language and Williams syndrome. Ann Rev Appl Linguist. 2008;28:191–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Mundy PC. Autism and joint attention: development, neuroscience, and clinical fundamentals. New York: Guilford Publications.

  60. Kuhl PK, Coffey‐Corina S, Padden D, Dawson G. Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Dev Sci. 2005;8(1):F1–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Nadig AS, Ozonoff S, Young GS, Rozga A, Sigman M, Rogers SJ. A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(4):378–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Paul R. Communication development and assessment. In: Chawarska K, Klin A, Volkmar F, editors. Autism spectrum disorders in infants and toddlers; 2008. p 76-103.

  63. Swensen LD, Kelley E, Fein D, Naigles LR. Processes of language acquisition in children with autism: evidence from preferential looking. Child Dev. 2007;78(2):542–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Tek S, Jaffery G, Fein D, Naigles LR. Do children with autism spectrum disorders show a shape bias in word learning? Autism Res. 2008;1(4):208–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Rhodes SM, Riby DM, Matthews K, Coghill DR. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and Williams syndrome: shared behavioral and neuropsychological profiles. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2011;33(1):147–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Keehn B, Nair A, Lincoln AJ, Townsend J, Müller RA. Under-reactive but easily distracted: an fMRI investigation of attentional capture in autism spectrum disorder. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2016;17:46–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;366(1567):1149–57.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Knutsen J, Mandell DS, Frye D. Children with autism are impaired in the understanding of teaching. Dev Sci. 2015. doi:10.1111/desc.12368.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge the children and parents involved in the study, the Victorian Autism Specific Early Learning and Care Centre Team who facilitated recruitment and testing of the ASD sample, the Williams Syndrome Family Support Group (Victoria), and the Williams Syndrome Association Australia. We would also like to acknowledge the valuable contribution of A/Prof Melanie Porter, Stephanie Sievers, Anna Atkinson, Jessica Reeve, Simone Griffith, Jacqueline Maya, and Cathriona Clarke.

Funding

This work was supported by Australian Research Council “near miss” funding and Research Focus Area Start-Up Project Grants from La Trobe University. DH was supported by an ARC Discovery Early Career Researcher Grant (DE160100042).

Availability of data and materials

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article will be uploaded in the Research Gate repository.

Authors’ contributions

All authors contributed intellectually to this work and read and approved the final draft of the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Approval for this research was granted by the La Trobe University Human Ethics Committee reference no. 14-007. Parents provided informed consent after reading a participant information sheet about the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Giacomo Vivanti.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vivanti, G., Hocking, D.R., Fanning, P. et al. Verbal labels increase the salience of novel objects for preschoolers with typical development and Williams syndrome, but not in autism. J Neurodevelop Disord 8, 46 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-016-9180-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-016-9180-7

Keywords