Skip to main content

Social visual attentional engagement and memory in Phelan-McDermid syndrome and autism spectrum disorder: a pilot eye tracking study

Abstract

Background

The current study used eye tracking to investigate attention and recognition memory in Phelan-McDermid syndrome (PMS), a rare genetic disorder characterized by intellectual disability, motor delays, and a high likelihood of comorbid autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Social deficits represent a core feature of ASD, including decreased propensity to orient to or show preference for social stimuli.

Methods

We used a visual paired-comparison task with both social and non-social images, assessing looking behavior to a novel image versus a previously viewed familiar image to characterize social attention and recognition memory in PMS (n = 22), idiopathic ASD (iASD, n = 38), and typically developing (TD) controls (n = 26). The idiopathic ASD cohort was divided into subgroups with intellectual disabilities (ID; developmental quotient < 70) and without (developmental quotient > 70) and the PMS group into those with and without a co-morbid ASD diagnosis.

Results

On measures of attention, the PMS group with a comorbid ASD diagnosis spent less time viewing the social images compared to non-social images; the rate of looking back and forth between images was lowest in the iASD with ID group. Furthermore, while all groups demonstrated intact recognition memory when novel non-social stimuli were initially presented (pre-switch), participants with PMS showed no preference during the post-switch memory presentation. In iASD, the group without ID, but not the group with ID, showed a novelty preference for social stimuli. Across indices, individuals with PMS and ASD performed more similarly to PMS without ASD and less similarly to the iASD group.

Conclusion

These findings demonstrate further evidence of differences in attention and memory for social stimuli in ASD and provide contrasts between iASD and PMS.

Background

Phelan-McDermid syndrome (PMS) is a rare developmental disorder that is characterized by broad cognitive delays, delayed or absent speech, and hypotonia [35, 38]. Caused by a deletion or mutation in the chromosome region 22q13.3, disruption of the SHANK3 gene causes the behavioral phenotype of the disorder [16, 22], with larger deletion sizes correlating with increased symptom severity [46, 48]. In animal models, deficient Shank3 expression results in atypical cognitive processes that include impaired visual discrimination and memory [14, 25] and diminished expressive communication [4, 655]), yet intact social approach behaviors [14, 17]. Many of these features are also seen in the clinical phenotype. Indeed, individuals with PMS show persistent communication deficits [34, 48, 60]. A majority have minimal or absent speech [37, 39], with intellectual disabilities (ID) reported in most cases [16, 48]. Additional behavioral characteristics include sleep disturbances, impulsivity, and inattention [8, 16, 27, 40]. In a subset of individual with PMS due to point mutations, De Rubeis et al. [16] reported that up to 65% are hyperactive, with broad attentional problems.

Studies have reported a high prevalence of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) diagnoses among individuals with PMS [31, 48]; in fact, SHANK3 haploinsufficiency is among the most common single-gene causes of ASD and ID [5, 28]. ASD is characterized by social and communication deficits as well as restricted interests and repetitive behaviors [2]. Though ASD is itself heterogeneous, within the social domain, these impairments can include poorly modulated eye contact, diminished awareness and understanding of the thoughts and intention of others, and difficulties in social engagement [50]. Specific to ASD symptoms, PMS individuals express deficits in social communication that involve social reciprocity, engagement, and play skills; up to 85% meet criteria for ASD [48]. However, in comparison to typical patterns within idiopathic ASD (iASD), a lower proportion of individuals with PMS have impairments in social approach and engagement behaviors, despite struggling with directing attention more than children with iASD [40]. Thus, while ASD is a clinical syndrome comprising a combination of features where social deficits are prominent, there are few studies that explore how deficits in specific social behaviors present in PMS.

It has been suggested that the behavioral manifestations of social dysfunction in ASD are the consequence of atypical attentional processing [12]. Typically developing infants display a sensitivity to social cues in their environment [49], and an attentional predisposition toward social stimuli is necessary for effective communication and language acquisition [13]. Individuals with ASD, however, often fail to orient to social stimuli [15] and also show an absence of preferential attention [24] and impaired recognition [7] particular to social input. In a visual paired-comparison task (VPC) that tested visual memory by pairing an unfamiliar stimulus with a familiar one, toddlers with ASD showed differential eye gaze patterns and reduced recognition memory for social stimuli compared to typically developing (TD) controls [9]. Here, preferential looking toward the novel stimuli infers recognition memory of the familiar stimulus [19]. Orienting to non-social stimuli, on the other hand, appears intact in ASD [15]. Together, these results reveal a reliable pattern of atypical attentional and memory processes for social information in ASD. However, it is unknown whether a similar pattern of visual attention and memory impairments specific to social stimuli is also present in PMS and how other clinical symptoms of PMS impact the presentation. The paucity of experimental data on PMS is in part attributable to the rare nature of the syndrome.

Moderate to profound ID affects approximately 75% of individuals with PMS [48]. Deficits in short-term memory are characteristic of a person with ID [1] and recognition memory indexed by visual attention preference scores measured with the VPC task in infants has shown a relationship to performance on standardized intelligence test later in life [45, 51]. Thus, clarifying whether potential deficits in visual attention and memory relate to the ASD phenotype or the presence of comorbid ID in a PMS sample is an important point of consideration.

The goal of the present study was to examine processing of social stimuli in PMS, in comparison to iASD and TD controls, using a visual paired-comparison task to test for syndrome-specific attentional and memory deficits of social information. We hypothesized, consistent with previous literature, that social attention and memory in iASD groups are atypical regardless of cognitive functioning, such that novelty preference for social stimuli would be reduced in comparison to non-social stimuli. We further expected that, relative to iASD groups, individuals with PMS would have a global deficit in memory and attention that was less specific to social recognition memory and more consistent with broad attentional and intellectual challenges. Finally, we predicted that individuals with PMS with comorbid ASD diagnoses would have social attention and memory profiles more similar to iASD than would individuals with PMS without an ASD diagnosis.

Method

Participants

Eye tracking data were collected from 85 5- to 18-year-old participants with iASD (n = 38), PMS (n = 22), and TD controls (n = 28). Following data cleaning (see below), the final sample consisted of 38 participants with iASD, 22 with PMS, and 26 TD controls. Legal guardians gave consent, and assent was obtained from participants when appropriate. Procedures were approved by the Program for the Protection of Human Subjects at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai.

For the iASD and PMS groups, clinical diagnosis was confirmed with the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2 [29];) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R [30];), followed by clinical consensus among licensed clinical psychologists and psychiatrists. All individuals in the iASD group met DSM-5 [2] criteria for ASD; in the PMS group, 14 participants (64%) met criteria for ASD. For the PMS group, chromosomal microarray or targeted sequencing was conducted to verify genetic diagnosis. Pathogenic deletion or mutation of the SHANK3 gene was necessary and sufficient for a PMS diagnosis. All individuals in the idiopathic ASD group had a negative array resulting from genetic testing. The PMS group was divided into participants with a co-morbid ASD diagnosis (PMS+; n = 14; females: 8 (57.1%)) and those without an ASD diagnosis (PMS; n = 8; females: 4 (50.0%)).

Participants were administered standardized measures of intellectual or developmental functioning. Depending on age and functioning level, participants received one of the following tests: the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales (SB-5 [41];), the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-V [57];), the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI-II [56];), the Differential Ability Scales (DAS, DAS-II [18];), or the Mullen Scales of Early Learning [33]. Developmental quotients [DQ: (mental age/chronological age * 100)] scores were computed from subtest-level age equivalent scores in order to combine data across the different IQ tests administered. The iASD group was divided by IQ to form a subgroup with intellectual disability to match with the PMS group, scoring DQ < 70 (iASD-lo; n = 7) and a second subgroup with DQ > 70 (iASD-hi, n = 31). This resulted in five groups: PMS+, PMS, iASD-lo, iASD-hi, and TD controls. Table 1 shows a summary of assessment scores.

Table 1 Participant demographics

Apparatus and testing procedure

An EyeLink 1000 plus eye-tracker in head-free mode with a 17-inch LCD monitor and 1280 × 1024 pixel at 32 bits per color display, with a refresh rate of 60 Hz was used for data collection. Data were acquired at 500 Hz with a 5- or 13-point calibration used on each participant before the start of the task.

Testing was performed in a dark, quiet room. Participants were positioned at a small table in either a chair or a booster seat roughly 50 cm from the eye tracker and monitor. Ambient sound and light were minimized in order to reduce distraction and tracking interference. The experimenter would give generic verbal instructions: “Look at the images you see on the screen. Between each set of images, look at a dot in the center of the screen.” Importantly, comprehension of these instructions was non-critical; participants were allowed to passively view on-screen images, and the trials were presented only when participants were fixating the screen (see below). In this way, the task was accessible to individuals with a wide range of cognitive and language abilities.

Stimuli and experimental procedure

The visual stimuli, from Rose et al. [42], comprised of achromatic faces (social) and multicolored abstract patterns (non-social) (see Fig. 1A), on a black background. At a viewing distance of 50 cm, stimuli subtended a 14.2° × 10.2° visual angle and the inner edges of the two images subtended 8.28° of visual angle. The task was presented using the EyeLink Experiment Builder software application.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A Sample of social (left) and non-social (right) visual stimuli and (B) trial schematic of visual paired-comparison task. Familiarization was followed by a pre-switch test, which featured a novel image, and finally post-switch test when the novel and familiar images swapped sides. The task was comprised of 9 trials that alternated between social and non-social trial types

This study used the well-established visual paired-comparison paradigm [20, 42]. Trials began with a familiarization period. During this phase, identical images were presented in pairs, one image on the left and one on the right of the vertical meridian. As per Rose et al. [42], viewing times were 10 s (social images) or 5 s (non-social images), excluding the time when the participant’s eye gaze left the display monitor. Prior research had found that these minimal familiarization periods were needed to achieve habituation effects [43]. Next, during the test period, the image displayed during familiarization was paired with a novel image for 5 s (pre-switch), after which the two images switched sides and were displayed for an additional 5 s (post-switch) (Fig. 1B). The familiarization and test periods were not blocked; rather, each trial began with a familiarization period that preceded the test period. Each trial featured a unique set of stimuli and the novel stimulus was never used as a stimulus during any of the familiarization periods. The location of the novel stimulus was balanced across stimulus types and test periods. As per Rose et al. [42], participants viewed nine trials in total: five social and four non-social. Order of social and non-social trials was fixed and alternated between the two trial types. A flashing target and a loud “spaceship” noise were shown for a few seconds between each trial to ensure participants returned to a central, neutral fixation point. The experimenter would visually confirm the position of the participant’s eye gaze before manually initiating the start of each trial.

Data analysis

Areas of interest (AOI) were defined as the rectangular area encompassing the image (AOI size, 14.2° × 10.2° of visual angle). Trials were excluded from analysis if familiarization/test period contained valid eye gaze for less than 25% of the total time within the AOI indicating that they were attending to the images less than 25% of the total time (social familiarization, 2.5 s; non-social familiarization, 1.25 s; test, 1.25 s). This threshold has been used previously in eye tracking studies [21]. Participants were excluded from analysis if they had less than two trials per condition. The trial minimum requirement used was based on infant literature where single trials are typical and sufficient for estimating looking behavior [10, 23]. Two TD participant met the exclusion criteria threshold (mean age, 7.11 (3.78), number female, 1 (50%), DQ, 133.23 (54.78)). There were no significant differences in the number of included trials among familiarization/test phases (F2,162 = 3.03, p = 0.051, ηp2 = 0.036; familiarization, 4.44 ± 0.025; pre-switch test, 4.37 ± 0.036; post-switch test, 4.36 ± 0.31), or by group (F4,81 = 1.53, p = 0.20, ηp2 = 0.07; TD, 4.47 ± 0.037; iASD-hi, 4.44 ± 0.34; iASD-lo, 4.38 ± 0.072; PMS, 4.33 ± 0.67; PMS+, 4.35 ± 0.051). There was a significant difference in the number of remaining trials by stimulus type (F1,81 = 1721.66, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.96; social, 4.89 ± 0.031; non-social, 3.9 ± 0.023) likely driven by the different number of possible trials for the two conditions (5 social and 4 non-social). None of the interaction reached statistical significance (period×group: F8,162 = 0.86, p = 0.57, ηp2 = 0.041; stimulus type×group: F1,81 = 0.65, p = 0.63, ηp2 = 0.031; period×stimulus type: F2,162 = 0.61, p = 0.54, ηp2 = 0.008; period×stimulus type×group: F2,162 = 0.53, p = 0.83, ηp2 = 0.026).

In familiarization, total visit duration (TVD) in AOIs, corrected for total display time difference of stimulus type, was calculated for both the left and right image, then summed to determine the proportion of total image exploration time. The data were subjected to a 2 × 5 mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) with stimulus type (social, non-social) as the within-subjects factor and diagnosis (iASD-hi, iASD-lo, PMS+, PMS, TD controls) as the between-subject factor. In addition, a rate of entering the AOI established the amount of switching back and forth between the two identical images and was analyzed with 2 × 2 × 5 mixed ANOVA with stimulus type, location (left, right), and diagnosis as levels of the independent variable (IV). The rate was calculated using the number of entries into the AOI divided by the exposure time (social, 10 s; non-social, 5 s).

During test, total visit duration for each image (novel and familiar) was calculated separately for the initial 5 s presentation (pre-switch) and after the images switched sides (post-switch). A preference score was calculated as the average time spent on the AOI for the familiar image subtracted from the time spent on the novel image divided by the sum of time spent on either image: (novel − familiar)/(novel + familiar). A score significantly different from zero in either direction (positive for novelty or negative for familiarity) was indicative of recognition memory. Preference scores were subjected to a 2 × 5 repeated measures ANOVA with switch period (pre-, post-), and diagnosis as IV levels for each stimulus type. One sample t tests were conducted separately for each clinical group to analyze the existence of a preference (i.e., significant difference from zero/no preference, where equal time is spent on the familiar and novel images).

All post hoc group comparisons were subjected to Games-Howell corrections for multiple comparisons to account for unequal variance. All other tests were Bonferroni corrected with a two-tailed 0.05 significance criterion.

Results

Clinical and demographics measures

The final sample showed no significant difference among groups for age, F4,52.11 = 0.55, p = 0.70, ηp2 = 0.025. There was a significant difference in DQ, F4,29.716 = 49.67, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.72, with eleven participants missing DQ scores (iASD-hi, 1; iASD-lo, 0; PMS+/−, 0; TD, 10). A clinician estimate of DQ score > 70 based on clinical impressions and patient history was determined for the one iASD-hi participant without a formal IQ assessment available. The mean DQ score of the TD control group was higher than the iASD-hi, iASD-lo, PMS+, and PMS groups (all ps < 0.04). The iASD-hi group also had significantly higher DQ scores compared to the PMS groups (p < 0.001 Games-Howell). However, the iASD-lo group did not differ from either PMS (p = 0.89 Games-Howell) or PMS+ (p = 0.063 Games-Howell) groups, and the PMS groups did not differ from one another in DQ (p = 0.18 Games-Howell). A significant sex difference was also found among the groups, χ (4) = 12.36, p = 0.015; a post hoc test of the adjusted residuals tested for groups statistically different from the expected equal distribution of sex across groups (i.e., iASDs=PMSs=TD). Here, it was revealed that the iASD-hi group (p = 0.002) was significantly different from the expected (null hypothesis) value of no difference in sex count. Follow-up comparisons using a two-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test found the iASD-hi group had a greater proportion of males when compared with the TD control group (p = 0.011); this did not survive significance after correcting for multiple comparisons. Nevertheless, this difference is consistent with the male:female ratio in ASD [3], whereas males and females are equally affected with PMS [36, 54]. No other comparisons for sex ratio were found to be significant after correcting for multiple comparisons (iASD-hi/iASD-lo, p = 1.00; iASD-hi/PMS, p = 1.00; iASD-hi/ PMS+, p = 0.96).

Familiarization

Attention to social and non-social stimuli was compared among groups during the familiarization period. There was a significant main effect of stimulus type (F1,81 = 4.16, p = 0.045, ηp2 = 0.049) and diagnostic group (F4,81 = 3.13, p = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.134), and a significant stimulus by diagnosis interaction (F4,81 = 2.87, p = 0.030, ηp2 = 0.12) on measures of proportion looking time. Across all groups, participants tended to look longer at non-social stimuli (0.67 ± 0.021) than looking at the social stimulus (0.64 ± 0.02). Between groups, no comparisons survived corrections for multiple comparison (p > 0.05, Games-Howell). The significant interaction effect was parsed into two separate ANOVAs by stimulus type. There was a significant group difference for social stimuli (F4,81 = 4.80, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.19), where the PMS+ group tended to look significantly less to the social images in comparison to the TD controls (p = 0.020). There were no significant group differences in looking behavior in the non-social images condition (F4,81 = 1.56, p = 0.19, ηp2 = 0.072). These findings indicate engagement with social images was less than engagement with non-social stimuli across groups and that, for social images, the PMS+ group engaged less than TD controls (Fig. 2A).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Familiarization results. A Proportion looking during social and non-social trial types for the iASD-hi (red, filled), iASD-lo (red, open), PMS (blue, filled) and PMS+ (blue, open), and TD controls (gray). B Average area of interest (AOI) image entry rate for social (right) and non-social (left) trial types averaged across location. The iASD-lo (red, open) had a lower AOI entry rate compared to the PMS+ (blue, open) and TD control group (gray). C Test results. top: pre-switch test period for social and non-social preference scores (red-filled, iASD-hi; red-open, iASD-lo; blue-filled, PMS; blue-open, PMS+; gray, TD controls) and bottom: post-switch test period. Values above zero denote a novelty preference and values below zero a familiarity preference. During the pre-switch period, all groups demonstrated a novelty preference for non-social images. In the post-switch period, the PMS subgroups and iASD-lo did not exhibit an image preference for social stimuli. Error bars represent ± 1 SE. *p < 0.5, **p < 0.01

To investigate differences in looking patterns, we analyzed the rate of saccade entries into the left and right images. We found a significant main effect for stimulus type (F1,81 = 18.23, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.18). Across all groups, participants had a higher rate of AOI entries to non-social stimuli (0.51 ± 0.02) than social stimuli (0.44 ± 0.02). There was no significant main effect of image location, i.e., left (0.47 ± 0.02) versus right (0.48 ± 0.02) image, F1,81 = 2.27, p = 0.14, ηp2 = 0.027. However, a statistically significant main effect of group (F4,81 = 3.66, p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.15) was detected wherein, overall, the iASD-lo group made less entries into the AOIs (0.34 ± 0.05) compared with the PMS+ (0.56 ± 0.03; p = 0.009, Games-Howell) and the TD control groups (0.49 ± 0.03; p = 0.043, Games-Howell). No other comparisons with the TD control group were found to be significant (TD/iASD-hi: p = 0.38; TD/PMS (0.51 ± 0.05): p = 0.99; TD/PMS+: p = 0.55). Comparisons with iASD-hi (0.43 ± 0.02) were neither significant (iASD-hi/iASD-lo, p = 0.54; iASD-hi/PMS, p = 0.77; iASD-hi/PMS+, p = 0.06) nor were the other comparisons with iASD-lo and PMS (p=0.37) or PMS and PMS+ (p = 0.94).

A significant group×stimulus type interaction (F1,80 = 4.60, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.19) was detected. Follow-up one sample t tests examining the AOI entry rate difference between non-social and social images (non-social—social) revealed that the TD control group showed the largest difference in AOI entry rate between social and non-social (mean difference, 0.17 ± 0.03; p < 0.001) stimuli favoring non-social images, followed by the iASD-hi group (0.06 ± 0.02; p = 0.006). The PMS groups showed neither significant difference between entry rate between stimulus types (PMS, −0.01 ± 0.05; p = 0.84; PMS+, 0.08 ± 0.04; p = 0.77) nor did the iASD-lo group (0.06 ± 0.04; p = 0.26). There was no significant group×location interaction, F4,81 = 1.75, p = 0.15, ηp2 = 0.1. No other interactions were significant (stimulus type×location: F1,81 = 0.29, p = 0.59, ηp2 = 0.004; group×stimulus type×location: F4,81 = 0.71, p = 0.59, ηp2 = 0.03) (Fig. 2B). In sum, engagement with stimuli differed among the groups in looking pattern by stimulus type in addition to overall time on screen.

Test

The test period introduced a novel image, evaluating memory through preferential looking toward the newly presented social and non-social stimuli. Due to the significant differences of attentional engagement between stimulus type, analyses were performed by social and non-social conditions separately. In the social conditions, we found a significant main effect of test period (pre-/post-switch: F1,81 = 39.17, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.33). Across groups, participants looked less at the novel image during the post-switch period (−0.066 ± 0.03) compared with the pre-switch test period (0.31 ± 0.3). There was no significant main effect of group (F4,81 = 1.50, p = 0.21, ηp2 = 0.07). There were no significant interactions between test period×group: F4,81 = 0.80, p = 0.53, ηp2 = 0.04).

An analysis of the non-social condition revealed similar results as for the social condition, namely, that there was a significant main effect of test period (F4,81 = 55.03, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.41), where there was a stronger novelty preference pre-switch (0.46 ± 0.03) than post-switch (−0.20 ± 0.03). As for the social condition, in the non-social condition, there was no significant main effect of group (F4,81 = 22.17, p = 0.080, ηp2 = 0.10) or an interaction effect (F4,81 = 0.62, p = 0.65, ηp2 = 0.03). In aggregate, these results indicate similarity among groups in their condition-specific recognition memory.

Finally, to test for the presence of a novelty/familiarity preference for each group and stimulus type, we ran one-sample t test on each group for each condition and test period (Fig. 2C). It was found that the TD control group had a novelty preference in the pre-switch test for both stimulus types (non-social, 0.52 ± 0.04, p < 0.001; social, 0.44 ± 0.04, p < 0.001). During the post-switch period, TD controls also demonstrated a novelty preference for non-social stimuli (0.28 ± 0.05, p < 0.001) and social stimuli (0.11 ± 0.05, p = 0.026). Similar to the TD control group, the iASD-hi group showed a novelty preference pre-switch across conditions (non-social, 0.52 ± 0.05; p < 0.001; social, 0.31 ± 0.05, p < 0.001) and a novelty preference post-switch for non-social ( 0.19 ± 0.04; p < 0.001) and social stimuli (0.12 ± 0.04, p = 0.012). The iASD-lo group displayed a novelty preference during the pre-switch period for non-social (0.51 ± 0.12, p = 0.005) but not for social (0.23 ± 0.11, p = 0.069) stimuli and similarly during post-switch (non-social, 0.33 ± 0.1, p = 0.004; social, 0.0005 ± 0.10, p = 1.00). The PMS group had a novelty preference pre-switch (non-social, 0.38 ± 0.06, p < 0.001; social, 0.31 ± 0.11, p = 0.221) and no preference post-switch (non-social, 0.10 ± 0.09, p =0.32; social, 0.06 ± 0.07, p = 0.037) as did the PMS+ group (pre-switch: social, 0.25 ± 0.05, p = 0.001; non-social, 0.36 ± 0.07, p < 0.001; post-switch: social, 0.031 ± 0.08, p = 0.70, non-social, 0.10 ± 0.09, p = 0.27). This last finding suggests decay of memory in iASD-lo and PMS groups by the post-switch interval.

There were no significant correlations between sex and the eye tracking measures of proportion looking (social, p = 0.70; non-social, p = 0.50) or rate of entry (social left, p = 0.17; social right, p = 0.30; non-social left, p = 0.39; non-social right, p = 0.88) during familiarization, or preferences scores during test (pre-switch social: p = 0.96; pre-switch non-social, p = 0.15; post-switch social, p = 0.15; post-switch non-social, p = 0.15).

Discussion

The present study in Phelan-McDermid syndrome examined two aspects of cognition that have been implicated in ASD: social attentional engagement and memory. This represents the first study to examine these constructs in PMS, one of the most common single-gene causes of ASD. We found that attentional engagement differed for social and non-social images for all groups and that individuals with PMS and a comorbid ASD diagnosis, but not those without comorbid ASD, had lower engagement compared to typically developing controls for social images specifically. In addition, the groups were dissociable in both their looking patterns and their preferential looking behavior toward novel versus familiar images. In particular, the iASD group with ID showed less active looking, as indexed by the entry rates, during stimulus familiarization compared to both PMS with an ASD diagnosis and typically developing controls. Interestingly, this pattern did not hold in participants with PMS without ASD who were similar in their cognitive functioning level. Finally, all groups showed a greater novelty preference for non-social (versus social) stimuli during the pre-switch test period. Results from the post-switch period revealed a novelty preference in both the autism group without ID and TD control group, while the iASD group with ID and both PMS groups demonstrated a lack of preference.

Our results in iASD are generally consistent with prior studies that report atypical processing of social stimuli in ASD. Namely, the lack of a novelty preference in the ASD with ID group on trials with social, but not non-social, images is in line with previous findings. A recent meta-analysis indicated that individuals with ASD are worse at remembering and discriminating facial identity [58] compared to TD controls, which is consistent with our findings. Some have attributed this domain-specific impairment for faces to reduced reward placed on social information [11]. Other research suggests that the complexity of faces presents a challenge for individuals with ASD in terms of discerning relevant and irrelevant information features, thereby affecting memory processes [53]. It is notable that within the iASD without ID group performance was comparable to typically developing controls. It cannot, however, be discounted that differences between social and non-social stimuli may have been in part been driven by the differences in the low-level features of the stimuli, such as color. While the stimuli were the same used in Rose et al. [42], the social stimuli were all gray scale images while the non-social images were in color. Past literature has found that color images are remembered more accurately than black and white images [59] and color images may have been interpreted as being more salient in attracting attention [52]. Future research would need to explore this further with stimuli better matched for low-level features.

During test (pre-switch), all groups except the iASD with ID group demonstrated a novelty preference across both stimulus types, indicating that participants were able to discriminate between the familiar and unfamiliar items. However, during post-switch, both PMS groups showed neither novelty nor familiarization, suggesting recognition memory had decayed over the 5 s pre-switch test period in these groups, whereas it remained intact for the TD control and both iASD groups. These results in PMS are consistent with studies of Shank3-deficient mice revealing specific deficits in the mediation of glutamatergic function [6], a neurotransmitter involved in learning and memory through long-term potentiation [32]. Individuals with ID consistently perform lower on assessments of memory ability than typically developing controls [47].

Interestingly, though the iASD group with ID showed a novelty-preference for non-social stimuli but not social, this condition-selective effect was not observed for ASD associated with PMS, wherein a novelty preference during initial presentation was observed for both social and non-social images. Participants with PMS and ASD looked more similar to participants with PMS without ASD, showing a novelty preference regardless of stimulus type. These findings suggest that the dissociation between social and non-social memory and novelty preference is less prominent in those with ASD caused by PMS relative to the typical profile observed in iASD more generally.

We found comparable viewing times among iASD, PMS without ASD, and TD control groups when participants were initially presented with identical image pairs during the familiarization phase. The lack of any differences suggests that each group had equitable cumulative looking times to become familiar with the images, which further suggests that differences in novelty preference were not due to differences in familiarity or attention. However, across groups, there were some notable differences in rates of looking away and returning to the image, which may measure the process of comparing the pair of images during initial exploration [44]. Those individuals with PMS and co-morbid ASD had the higher rates of looking between stimuli, whereas the iASD group with ID was significantly lower by comparison. The higher switching rate paired with the lower dwell time during familiarization for the PMS with ASD group suggest that the going back and forth between images impacted total viewing time, but did not have significant consequences on recognition memory during the initial test. Here again, results highlight a pattern of dissociable social impairments in ASD associated with PMS versus idiopathic ASD more broadly. In the iASD without ID and TD control groups, there was a tendency for more active looking at non-social versus social images. In the PMS without ASD group, overall rates of looking between images were comparable to the TD control group, but the difference in looking rates for social versus non-social stimuli observed in the other two groups was absent. These results could suggest that looking behaviors such as rate of entry, more than looking time, contribute to subsequent differences in memory and preference for social and non-social stimuli among groups. Previous research has suggested a high level of switching back and forth between items was correlated with better memory performance [26, 44]; why the PMS with ASD group shows intact rates of looking switches yet poorer memory by the post-test interval could be attributed to deficient long-term potentiation [6], in spite of intact initial encoding behavior and early (pre-switch) memory.

While the switch during the test period was introduced to control for side preferences, the difference in performance (a weaker novelty preference overall in the post-switch period) between the pre- and post-switch periods suggests that participants were not acting the same across the test periods. One possibility is that the abrupt change in images during the switch could be interpreted by some participants as a novelty of location, or memory was beginning to decay over time.

Study limitations are due primarily to factors inherent in studying rare disease populations. First, groups were not matched on DQ scores. ID is characteristic of the vast majority of individuals with PMS and we attempted to control for DQ differences by having an iASD group with ID that was comparable in DQ. The visual paired-comparison paradigm has been used successfully with infants as young as 5 months old [43], has minimal instructions, and is free-viewing in nature, which ought to make it suitable for all our groups, regardless of intellectual functioning. Furthermore, we found no group differences in looking time during image familiarization suggesting the PMS and iASD with ID groups were broadly attentive to the task, with their eye gaze on the display monitor. Despite these considerations, we cannot discount that memory performance in the PMS groups may be associated more with their intellectual disabilities than their ASD phenotype. These groups tended to have comparable performance despite differing on ASD status and performed more similar to the iASD group with ID, with whom they have ID in common. While this study is among the first to probe specific cognitive functioning in a task-based way, it will be important for future studies to IQ match and examine the drivers of this PMS phenotype, whether it be ID or ASD. In addition, including non-PMS ID groups without ASD for comparison would help clarify this issue. Second, our study included participants across a wide age range in order to recruit an adequate sample size given the rarity of PMS. However, the rarity of this disorder—and of empirical research describing core cognitive deficits—emphasizes its uniqueness and the importance of this work despite some sample limitations. Lastly, while stimuli differed across trials and between stimulus types, the images were taken from previously published studies and were calibrated for equal attractiveness within a trial as well as for habituation times reported for social and non-social images [42, 43]; however, it remains possible that differences between calibrated stimuli across social and non-social images and their presentation times could impact results in addition to the differences in the number of trials presented. In strictly following the experimental design of Rose et al. [42], we sought to replicate an established task with published results as closely as possible. As this study involved participants with a rare disorder who are difficult to recruit and test, grounding our experiment in previously published study was particularly important for providing background against which to contextualize our results.

Conclusion

In summary, while we found no general attentional engagement impairments in iASD or PMS, there were signs of disorder and domain-specific memory impairments and looking patterns. We found that, overall, individuals with PMS show a novelty preference regardless of stimulus type, whereas individuals with iASD matched in cognitive functioning showed only a novelty preference for non-social stimuli. In addition, both looking behaviors and novelty preference differed between iASD and ASD in the context of PMS. These unique patterns of social attentional engagement and memory in PMS differentiate it from patterns typical of iASD and could both clarify underlying mechanistic alterations and inform treatment targets for PMS. In addition, searching for PMS-like attention and memory patterns could serve as a way to stratify individuals within heterogeneous iASD samples and potentially apply knowledge gained in PMS to these iASD subsets as well.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

References

  1. Alloway TP. Working memory and executive function profiles of individuals with borderline intellectual functioning. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54(5):448–56.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®): American Psychiatric Pub; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Baio J, Wiggins L, Christensen DL, Maenner MJ, Daniels J, Warren Z, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2018;67(6):1.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Berg EL, Copping NA, Rivera JK, Pride MC, Careaga M, Bauman MD, et al. Developmental social communication deficits in the Shank3 rat model of Phelan-Mcdermid syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2018;11(4):587–601.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Betancur C, Buxbaum JD. SHANK3 haploinsufficiency: a “common” but underdiagnosed highly penetrant monogenic cause of autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism. 2013;4:17.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Bozdagi O, Sakurai T, Papapetrou D, Wang X, Dickstein DL, Takahashi N, et al. Haploinsufficiency of the autism-associated Shank3 gene leads to deficits in synaptic function, social interaction, and social communication. Mol Autism. 2010;1(1):15.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Bradshaw J, Shic F, Chawarska K. Brief report: face-specific recognition deficits in young children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41(10):1429–35.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Bro D, O’Hara R, Primeau M, Hanson-Kahn A, Hallmayer J, Bernstein JA. Sleep disturbances in individuals with Phelan-McDermid syndrome: correlation with caregivers’ sleep quality and daytime functioning. Sleep. 2017;40(2):zsw062.

  9. Chawarska K, Shic F. Looking but not seeing: atypical visual scanning and recognition of faces in 2 and 4-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(12):1663.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Cheries EW, Wynn K, Scholl BJ. Interrupting infants’ persisting object representations: an object-based limit? Dev Sci. 2006;9(5):F50–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(4):231–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Chita-Tegmark M. Social attention in ASD: a review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Res Dev Disabil. 2016;48:79–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(4):148–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Copping NA, Berg EL, Foley GM, Schaffler MD, Onaga BL, Buscher N, et al. Touchscreen learning deficits and normal social approach behavior in the Shank3B model of Phelan–McDermid syndrome and autism. Neuroscience. 2017;345:155–65.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Dawson G, Toth K, Abbott R, Osterling J, Munson J, Estes A, et al. Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Dev Psychol. 2004;40(2):271.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. De Rubeis S, Siper PM, Durkin A, Weissman J, Muratet F, Halpern D, et al. Delineation of the genetic and clinical spectrum of Phelan-McDermid syndrome caused by SHANK3 point mutations. Mol Autism. 2018;9(1):31.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Drapeau E, Riad M, Kajiwara Y, Buxbaum JD. Behavioral phenotyping of an improved mouse model of Phelan-McDermid syndrome with a complete deletion of the Shank3 gene. eNeuro. ENEURO-0046. 2018.

  18. Elliot CD. Differential ability scales–second edition: introductory and technical handbooks; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Fagan JF III. Memory in the infant. J Exp Child Psychol. 1970;9(2):217–26.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Fagan JF. The paired-comparison paradigm and infant intelligence. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1990;608(1):337–64.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Fischer J, Koldewyn K, Jiang YV, Kanwisher N. Unimpaired attentional disengagement and social orienting in children with autism. Clin Psychol Sci. 2014;2(2):214–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613496242.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Harony-Nicolas H, De Rubeis S, Kolevzon A, Buxbaum JD. Phelan McDermid syndrome: from genetic discoveries to animal models and treatment. J Child Neurol. 2015;30(14):1861–70.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Káldy Z, Leslie AM. Identification of objects in 9-month-old infants: integrating ‘what’and ‘where’information. Dev Sci. 2003;6(3):360–73.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Klin A, Lin DJ, Gorrindo P, Ramsay G, Jones W. Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature. 2009;459(7244):257.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Kouser M, Speed HE, Dewey CM, Reimers JM, Widman AJ, Gupta N, et al. Loss of predominant Shank3 isoforms results in hippocampus-dependent impairments in behavior and synaptic transmission. J Neurosci. 2013;33(47):18448–68.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Kovack-Lesh KA, Horst JS, Oakes LM. The cat is out of the bag: the joint influence of previous experience and looking behavior on infant categorization. Infancy. 2008;13(4):285–307.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Laura P, Marie G, Romuald B, Catherine B, Sylvie R, Arnold M, et al. 22q13 deletion syndrome: communication disorder or autism? Evidence from a specific clinical and neurophysiological phenotype. Transl Psychiatry. 2018;8(1):146.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Leblond CS, Nava C, Polge A, Gauthier J, Huguet G, Lumbroso S, et al. Meta-analysis of SHANK mutations in autism spectrum disorders: a gradient of severity in cognitive impairments. PLoS Genet. 2014;10(9):e1004580.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule–2nd edition (ADOS-2). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Corporation; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24(5):659–85.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Manning MA, Cassidy SB, Clericuzio C, Cherry AM, Schwartz S, Hudgins L, et al. Terminal 22q deletion syndrome: a newly recognized cause of speech and language disability in the autism spectrum. Pediatrics. 2004;114(2):451–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. McEntee WJ, Crook TH. Glutamate: its role in learning, memory, and the aging brain. Psychopharmacology. 1993;111(4):391–401.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Mullen EM. Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines: AGS; 1995. p. 58–64.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Oberman LM, Boccuto L, Cascio L, Sarasua S, Kaufmann WE. Autism spectrum disorder in Phelan-McDermid syndrome: initial characterization and genotype-phenotype correlations. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2015;10(1):105.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Phelan MC. Deletion 22q13. 3 syndrome. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2008;3(1):14.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Phelan K, Rogers RC, Boccuto L. Phelan-McDermid syndrome. In: GeneReviews®[internet]. Seattle: University of Washington; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Phelan MC, Rogers RC, Saul RA, Stapleton GA, Sweet K, McDermid H, et al. 22q13 deletion syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 2001;101(2):91–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Phelan K, McDermid HE. The 22q13. 3 deletion syndrome (Phelan-McDermid syndrome). Mol Syndromol. 2011;2(3-5):186–201.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Rankine J, Li E, Lurie S, Rieger H, Fourie E, Siper PM, et al. Language ENvironment analysis (LENA) in Phelan-McDermid syndrome: validity and suggestions for use in minimally verbal children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47(6):1605–17.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Richards C, Powis L, Moss J, Stinton C, Nelson L, Oliver C. Prospective study of autism phenomenology and the behavioural phenotype of Phelan–McDermid syndrome: comparison to fragile X syndrome, Down syndrome and idiopathic autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord. 2017;9(1):37.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Roid GH. Stanford-Binet intelligence scale: Riverside Publishing; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Rose SA, Djukic A, Jankowski JJ, Feldman JF, Fishman I, Valicenti-Mcdermott M. Rett syndrome: an eye-tracking study of attention and recognition memory. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(4):364–71.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Rose SA, Feldman JF, Jankowski JJ. Attention and recognition memory in the 1st year of life: a longitudinal study of preterm and full-term infants. Dev Psychol. 2001a;37(1):135.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Rose SA, Feldman JF, Jankowski JJ. Infant visual recognition memory: independent contributions of speed and attention. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(3):563.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Rose SA, Feldman JF, Wallace IF. Individual differences in infants’ information processing: reliability, stability, and prediction. Child Dev. 1988:1177–97.

  46. Sarasua SM, Dwivedi A, Boccuto L, Rollins JD, Chen CF, Rogers RC, et al. Association between deletion size and important phenotypes expands the genomic region of interest in Phelan–McDermid syndrome (22q13 deletion syndrome). J Med Genet. 2011;48(11):761–6.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Schuchardt K, Gebhardt M, Mäehler C. Working memory functions in children with different degrees of intellectual disability. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54(4):346–53.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Soorya L, Kolevzon A, Zweifach J, Lim T, Dobry Y, Schwartz L, et al. Prospective investigation of autism and genotype-phenotype correlations in 22q13 deletion syndrome and SHANK3 deficiency. Mol Autism. 2013;4(1):18.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Striano T, Reid VM. Social cognition in the first year. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(10):471–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Tager-Flusberg H. The origins of social impairments in autism spectrum disorder: studies of infants at risk. Neural Netw. 2010;23(8-9):1072–6.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Thompson LA, Fagan JF, Fulker DW. Longitudinal prediction of specific cognitive abilities from infant novelty preference. Child Dev. 1991;62(3):530–8.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Turatto M, Galfano G. Color, form and luminance capture attention in visual search. Vis Res. 2000;40(13):1639–43.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Van de Cruys S, Evers K, Van der Hallen R, Van Eylen L, Boets B, de Wit L, et al. Precise minds in uncertain worlds: predictive coding in autism. Psychol Rev. 2014;121(4):649.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Wang AT, Lim T, Jamison J, Bush L, Soorya LV, Tavassoli T, et al. Neural selectivity for communicative auditory signals in Phelan-McDermid syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2016;8(1):5.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Wang X, McCoy PA, Rodriguiz RM, Pan Y, Je HS, Roberts AC, et al. Synaptic dysfunction and abnormal behaviors in mice lacking major isoforms of Shank3. Hum Mol Genet. 2011;20(15):3093–108.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence–second edition (WASI-II). San Antonio: NCS Pearson; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children–fifth edition (WISC-V): technical and interpretive manual. Bloomington: Pearson Clinical Assessment; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Weigelt S, Koldewyn K, Kanwisher N. Face identity recognition in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(3):1060–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Wichmann FA, Sharpe LT, Gegenfurtner KR. The contributions of color to recognition memory for natural scenes. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2002;28(3):509.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Zwanenburg RJ, Ruiter SA, van den Heuvel ER, Flapper BC, Van Ravenswaaij-Arts CM. Developmental phenotype in Phelan-McDermid (22q13. 3 deletion) syndrome: a systematic and prospective study in 34 children. J Neurodev Disord. 2016;8(1):16.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge the children and families who participated in this project.

Funding

This research was supported in part by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences TL1TR001434, the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant U54NS092090, and the Seaver Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AK, PMS, ATW, JHF, MPT, DBH, and JDB contributed to conceptualization and participated in the methodology and supervision. All authors performed data collection, formal analysis, and investigation. JHF, SBG, and VZB conceived the writing—original draft preparation. All authors performed the writing—review and editing. AK and JDB contributed to funding acquisition and resources.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer H. Foss-Feig.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the Mount Sinai Program for the Protection of Human Subjects. All participants received informed consent about the purpose of the research project, possible risks and benefits, and the contact information of the researcher and the institutional program for the Protection of Human Subjects Office.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

AK receives research support from AMO Pharma and consults to Acadia, Ovid Therapeutics, and sema4. All other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Guillory, S.B., Baskett, V.Z., Grosman, H.E. et al. Social visual attentional engagement and memory in Phelan-McDermid syndrome and autism spectrum disorder: a pilot eye tracking study. J Neurodevelop Disord 13, 58 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-021-09400-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-021-09400-2

Keywords