Skip to main content

Altered sensitivity to social gaze in the FMR1 premutation and pragmatic language competence

Abstract

Background

The FMR1 premutation affects 1:291 women and is associated with a range of cognitive, affective, and physical health complications, including deficits in pragmatic language (i.e., social language). This study investigated attention to eye gaze as a fundamental social-cognitive skill that may be impaired in the FMR1 premutation and could underlie pragmatic deficits. Given the high prevalence of the FMR1 premutation, efforts to define its phenotype and mechanistic underpinnings have significant public health implications.

Methods

Thirty-five women with the FMR1 premutation and 20 control women completed an eye-tracking paradigm that recorded time spent dwelling within the eye region in response to a face displaying either direct or averted gaze. Pragmatic language ability was coded from a conversational sample using the Pragmatic Rating Scale.

Results

Women with the FMR1 premutation failed to show attentional preference to direct gaze and spent more time dwelling on the averted eyes relative to controls. While dwelling on the eyes was associated with better pragmatic language performance in controls, these variables were unrelated in the women with the FMR1 premutation.

Conclusions

Altered sensitivity to social gaze, characterized by increased salience of averted gaze, was observed among women with the FMR1 premutation. Furthermore, women with the FMR1 premutation were unable to capitalize on information conveyed through the eyes to enhance social-communicative engagement, which differed from patterns seen in controls. These findings contribute to the growing characterization of social and communication phenotypes associated with the FMR1 premutation.

Background

The Fragile X Mental Retardation-1 (FMR1) premutation is a prevalent genetic alteration affecting 1 in 291 women [1]. The premutation occurs when the CGG element on the 5′ untranslated region of the FMR1 expands within the range of 55–200 repeats [2]. The expanded CGG sequence shows generational instability and women “carriers” of the FMR1 premutation are at risk of passing the mutation to their offspring, which causes fragile X syndrome when the expansion exceeds 200 repeats [3].

Besides conferring familial risk, the FMR1 premutation is associated with its own clinical consequences, which include increased risk for mental health disorders [4], mild cognitive deficits [5], and physical health complications such as fragile X-associated premature ovarian insufficiency and fragile X-associated tremor ataxia syndrome (FXTAS; [6, 7]). Much existing research on the FMR1 premutation has focused on men, who show social-cognitive difficulties such as decreased ability to “read” mental and emotional states from the eyes [8]. Atypical brain and autonomic responses to social stimuli, such as reduced conductance response when greeting an unfamiliar person and diminished amygdala activation when viewing fearful faces, are also seen in males with the premutation [9, 10]. Less research has focused on females, but emerging work suggests a similar profile of social-cognitive weaknesses. For instance, risk for autism is increased among females with the premutation, with about 5% meeting criteria for autism spectrum disorder [11]. Women with the FMR1 premutation also exhibit increased personality features consistent with the broad autism phenotype [12] and decreased sensitivity to biological motion [13]. Yet, the phenotypic signature of the FMR1 premutation is not fully understood, and few investigations have focused on language features in this group.

Difficulty with pragmatic language (i.e., social language) is a recently documented feature of the FMR1 premutation that is prime for further study, given its clinical relevance. As a group, women with the FMR1 premutation commit more pragmatic language violations during conversation than do control women, which includes features such as over-talkativeness and introducing inappropriate topics [12]. At the family level, these features are clinically significant; pragmatic language difficulties among mothers with the FMR1 premutation are associated with poor language outcomes for their children with fragile X syndrome [14]. Additionally, these pragmatic language features may have negative consequences at the individual level. Pragmatic impairments are associated with lower quality friendships [15], social rejection [16], emotional difficulties [17], and feelings of loneliness [18] in other clinical groups, although individual effects have not yet been studied in the premutation.

While impaired use of eye gaze to infer mental/emotional states has been documented among men with the premutation (i.e., [8]), social-cognitive abilities involving the eye gaze have not been investigated among women with the FMR1 premutation. The present study sought to address this gap in the literature, with a focus on differential sensitivity to direct and averted gaze. The eyes represent a powerful modality for gleaning information about the social world, lending clues into the thoughts, beliefs, desires, and emotional state of others [19]. Sensitivity to the direction of eye gaze is hypothesized to represent a biologically prepared skill, as very young infants exhibit enhanced neural processing in response to direct gaze and are able to distinguish direct from averted gaze [20, 21]. This innate sensitivity to eye gaze is crucial to the development of theory of mind, or the ability to attribute mental states to others, and is thought to provide the foundation for the later development of social and communication skills [20, 22]. Human preference for direct gaze over averted gaze is well documented, with more efficient detection of direct gaze during visual search tasks [23, 24] and longer disengagement latencies in response to direct gaze [25]. The increased saliency of direct gaze has evolutionary significance, as direct gaze represents a primary communicative channel by which humans exchange social-cognitive information, facilitating adaptive social engagement [26]. Consistent with this notion, research shows that direct gaze facilitates the retrieval of social-cognitive knowledge, with enhanced recognition of emotions, faces, speech, and gender observed under direct gaze conditions relative to averted gaze [2733]. In sum, the eyes, and the direction of eye gaze in particular, represent a valuable source of information needed for social approach and engagement.

A number of empirical studies support a role of social gaze in language acquisition [3437]. According to the social-pragmatic theory, social-cognitive skills, such as the ability to “read the mind from the eyes,” are fundamental to language development because they allow communicative partners to establish a shared context that can be used to infer the meaning behind words and utterances [38, 39]. Eye gaze in particular represents a medium through which communication partners exchange critical information about intentions, moods, and emotions [40, 41]. During conversation, conversational partners spend a great deal of time attending closely to others’ eye gaze patterns [4244]. Eye gaze provides information on speaker intent and can be useful in resolving conversational ambiguities [45] and in detecting deception [46]. Information on the conversational partner’s interest and understanding is also conveyed through gaze, allowing the speaker to adopt their message to enhance the listener’s engagement and comprehension [43, 47]. Eye gaze is also used to establish listener/speaker roles so that disruptive turn-taking, such as interruptions and overlapping speech, are kept at a minimum [48, 49]. Thus, suboptimal use of gaze signals can lead to a number of maladaptive conversational strategies. This study tested the hypothesis that aberrant attention to social gaze contributes to pragmatic deficits in the FMR1 premutation.

The present study

This study employed an eye-tracking paradigm to record the time spent dwelling on the eyes in response to an animated face displaying either direct or averted gaze. The study’s first objective was to determine whether attention to the eyes in response to direct gaze and averted gaze, reflecting perceived salience, differed across women with the FMR1 premutation and control women. The second aim was to determine whether attention to the eyes under direct and averted gaze conditions was associated with pragmatic language variation in these groups. The FMR1 premutation is a highly prevalent genetic condition, making efforts to define its phenotype and associated mechanisms a public health imperative. This work is also important because of its implications for understanding the underpinnings of language behaviors relevant to both atypical and typical populations as they present during adulthood—an understudied and underserved developmental period. The following research questions were tested:

  1. 1.

    Does attention to the eyes in response to direct and averted gaze differ in women with the FMR1 premutation contrasted to control women? It was hypothesized that women with the FMR1 premutation would spend less time dwelling on the eyes than control women, reflecting social-cognitive weaknesses in this group. It was hypothesized that both groups would dwell on the eyes longer in the direct gaze condition than in the averted gaze condition, consistent with evidence supporting preferential attention to direct gaze.

  2. 2.

    Is attention to the eyes in response to direct and averted gaze associated with pragmatic language variation in women with the FMR1 premutation contrasted to control women? Consistent with the notion that eye gaze is a necessary source of information for social engagement, it was hypothesized that dwelling on the eyes in response to direct and averted gaze would be associated with increased pragmatic language competence in both groups.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-five women with the FMR1 premutation and 20 control women participated. Participants were drawn from a larger study of social-language in women with the FMR1 premutation that has been previously described (Klusek et al., [50]). All participants were native speakers of English, were mothers, had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, and did not have an intellectual disability as defined by an IQ composite >80 on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test-II [51]. Women with the FMR1 premutation were recruited through their children who were participating in developmental studies of children with the fragile X full mutation or the premutation (PI’s: Abbeduto, Roberts) or were recruited through the local community through word of mouth targeting mothers of children with fragile X or the premutation. Recruitment was based in the Eastern and Midwestern regions of the United States of America through advertisements on social media, parent support networks, and with the assistance of the Research Participant Registry Core of the Carolina Institute for Developmental Disabilities. The FMR1 premutation was confirmed via genetic testing and defined by an allele ranging from 55 to 200 CGG repeats on FMR1. None of the women with the premutation had been diagnosed with FXTAS, per self-report, and functional symptoms of tremor were screened out using the Tremor Disability Questionnaire [52]. Control women had no known family history of fragile X and were mothers of typically developing children (i.e., children who had not been diagnosed or treated for any type of developmental delay or disorder, per participant report). Although it was beyond the scope of the present study to conduct genotyping on controls, 85% of the control sample completed blood tests to rule out the FMR1 premutation through dual enrollment in a related pilot study. Women in the control group were excluded from the study if their child scored above the cut-off for autism spectrum disorder on the Social Communication Questionnaire (n = 1) [53]. The groups did not differ significantly on age, IQ, race, or education level; see Table 1.

Table 1 Group characteristics

Procedure

Assessments were conducted at a university laboratory setting, within the context of a larger research protocol that lasted approximately 3 h. Participants were compensated $50.00 for their time. Informed consent was obtained, and all procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of South Carolina.

Eye gaze paradigm

Participants were seated in a quiet, well-lit room. Eye movements were recorded using a desktop mounted Eyelink 1000 Plus (SR Research Ltd., Ontario, Canada) operating in remote mode, which provides compensation for head movements. Eye movements were sampled at 500 Hz, and tracking was monocular. The experiment was programed and run using the Experiment Builder software (version 1.10 SR Research Ltd., Ontario, Canada). Saccades and fixations were parsed based on the online velocity and acceleration of the eye using default parameters built into the EyeLink 1000 Plus software. Stimuli were presented on a BEN-Q 2420T monitor, displayed at the resolution of 1366 × 768 pixels and a 60 Hz refresh rate. The size of the screen was 530 × 300 mm, viewed from a distance of 950 mm (allowing for a maximum visual angle of 31° in the horizontal dimension and 18° in the vertical dimension). An initial 5-point calibration and validation were performed at the start of the procedure. Calibrations were not accepted until average, and maximum errors were less than .5° and 1.00°, respectively. Each trial was preceded by a centrally presented single point drift check that recorded any drift in the eye data before the trial began. Recalibrations were conducted as needed to prevent drift.

Stimuli and procedures were adapted from Weiser et al. [54]. The eye gaze paradigm consisted of a series of short video sequences where a computer-generated female face (which subtended 7° in the horizontal and 14° in the vertical dimension) started with her eyes closed and then opened her eyes to display either direct or averted gaze (see Fig. 1, for example, stimuli). At the start of the task, participants were instructed: “You are going to see a series of faces on the screen. The faces will have their eyes closed. I want you to look within the eye region of the face until the eyes open. After the eyes open, you can look anywhere you want on the screen.” Each trial initially depicted the face with the eyes closed for a minimum of 1200 ms. Once the participant was looking within the eye region for an additional 300 consecutive milliseconds (resulting in a minimum eyes-closed duration of 1500 ms), the eyes opened. After the eyes opened, the face remained in the direct or averted gaze position for 6000 ms. Individual trials were separated by an inter-trial-interval ranging from 300 to 400 ms, which consisted of a blank screen. The female faces were displayed on a gray background and showed a neutral facial expression. Stimuli were presented so that the eye region area of interest (defined by a rectangle drawn around the eyes which subtended 8° in the horizontal and 3° in the vertical dimension) was centered on the screen. Overall, 16 different female characters were used, each displaying three possible gaze conditions: direct, averted to the left, or averted to the right. Stimuli for each condition (direct, averted) were displayed at random until 16 trials of each condition had been shown. In the averted condition, the direction of eye gaze (right, left) was randomly selected. Only data from the first 16 trials of each condition were analyzed, yielding a total of 32 trials. Dataviewer (version 2.41 SR Research, Ontario, Canada) was used to extract the variables of interest. Dataviewer loads all of the online-parsed fixations, saccades, blinks, and samples. Eye movement data is then mapped to the experiment interest areas and images for more detailed analyses. Eye movement data were analyzed in terms of the percent dwell time on the eye interest area across all trials within each condition. On each trial, the cumulative fixation duration (i.e., excluding time spent making saccades and blinks) falling within the eye interest area was divided by the cumulative fixation duration across all fixations in the trial. This proportion was then multiplied by 100 to indicate the percentage of total fixation time spent on the eye region.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Example stimuli from the eye gaze paradigm. Note. Example stimuli from the eye gaze paradigm are presented. Trials begin with the eyes closed (leftmost image). After the eyes open, the face displays either direct gaze (top right) or averted gaze (bottom right). The eye area of interest is marked with a white rectangle

Pragmatic language assessment

Pragmatic language ability was rated from a 20-min conversational sample in which participants conversed with an examiner about their “life history.” Standard probe questions were used to elicit conversation on neutral, shared experiences, such as “What activities did you enjoy most as a child” and “Did you participate in extra-curricular activities in high school?” In addition to administering the standard probe questions, examiners commented, offered information, and asked follow-up questions to ensure ample opportunities for conversational exchange. Pragmatic ability was coded from the videotaped conversational samples using the modified version of the Pragmatic Rating Scale [55], which has been described previously as a tool for capturing subclinical pragmatic difficulties in relatives of children with developmental disorders (see [12, 56]). The scale consists of 26 items that are coded based on operational definitions capturing the frequency and severity of each potential violation, where generally a “0” denotes the violation is absent, a “1,” mild or questionable, and a “2,” present and striking. Example items include “fails to provide background information,” “pedantic word choice,” and “unusual intonation.” A total score denoting the severity of pragmatic language difficulties is computed by tallying the items. Two trained independent raters coded each sample and disagreements were resolved through discussion. Consensus codes were used in the analysis. The second rater was blind to the group membership of all participants, whereas the first rater (JK) was unable to remain blinded due to her role in participant assessment. Inter-rater reliability of the total score prior to consensus was ICC (3, 2) = 0.73 which is considered “good” [57].

Data analysis

Analyses were conducted in SAS 9.4 [58]. The data distribution was first examined, and all variables were normally distributed. To determine group differences in the percent dwell time on the eyes across direct and averted gaze conditions (Research Question 1), a mixed effects linear model was fit to test for group differences in the percent of time dwelling within the eye region of the face across conditions. Condition (averted or direct gaze) was specified as a random effect, nested within participant. Group, condition, and their interaction were included as predictors. An unstructured covariance matrix was specified. Analyses were conducted to determine whether the percent dwell time on the eyes across conditions predicted pragmatic language ability across the groups (Research Question 2). General linear models were conducted to test dwell time, group, and their interaction as predictors of the pragmatic language total score. Separate models were run for the averted and direct gaze conditions, and the Benjamini-Hochberg correction procedure [59] was used to control for false discovery by adjusting critical values for the model F test. Interaction contrasts were calculated to determine the effect of dwell time on pragmatic language competency at each level of group. Partial eta squared (η 2 p) effect sizes were computed. In general, values of η 2 p at 0.01, 0.06, and 0.14 are considered “small,” “medium,” and “large,” respectively [60].

Results

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive statistics for the predictor and outcome variables are reported in Table 2. The groups differed significantly on the pragmatic language total score, t (39.62) = 3.57, p = .001, with elevated pragmatic language violations in the women with the FMR1 premutation relative to controls.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics

Group differences in time dwelling on the eyes

The mixed effects model showed a significant main effect for condition (F [1, 53] = 5.22, p = .026) and a non-significant main effect for group (F [1, 53] = 0.11 p = .744). A significant group-by-condition interaction was detected, F (1, 53) = 5.51, p = .023. The control women spent less time dwelling on the eyes in the averted gaze condition relative to relative to direct gaze, whereas the dwell times of the women with the FMR1 premutation did not vary by condition. Relative to the controls, the women with the FMR1 premutation spent significantly more time dwelling on the eyes in the averted condition, whereas groups spent a similar amount of time dwelling on the eyes in the direct gaze condition (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Time dwelling on the eyes across groups and conditions

Association between time dwelling on the eyes and pragmatic language ability

Direct gaze model

The combined influence of group, time dwelling on the eyes in the direct gaze condition, and their interaction accounted for significant variance in pragmatic language ability, F (3, 50) = 7.11, p < .001, R 2 = .30. The group-by-condition interaction term was significant (p = .016), with a η 2 p of 0.11 consistent with a medium-to-large effect (Cohen, 1969). Interaction contrasts indicated that the effect of dwell time in the direct gaze condition on pragmatic language ability differed by group; among the control women, increased dwell time on the eyes was significantly associated with better pragmatic language skills (F [1, 53] = 6.04, p = .018, η 2 p = .11 [90% CI = .01–.24]), whereas the time dwelling on the eyes was not associated with pragmatic ability in the FMR1 premutation (F [1, 53] = 1.19, p = .280, η 2 p = .02 [90% CI = 0–.12]), see Fig. 3.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Association between time dwelling on the eyes during the direct gaze condition and pragmatic language ability. Note. A higher total score on the Pragmatic Rating Scale denotes increased pragmatic language difficulties

Averted gaze model

Findings for the averted gaze model were similar, with the combined influence of group, time dwelling on the eyes in the averted condition, and their interaction accounting for significant variance in pragmatic language ability, F (3, 50) = 6.18, p = .001, R 2 = .27. A significant group-by-condition interaction was detected (p = .039), with an η 2 p of 0.08 consistent with a medium effect (Cohen, 1969). Similar to the direct gaze model, interaction contrasts indicated that the influence of dwell time on pragmatics differed by group membership; increased dwell time in the averted condition was significantly associated with better pragmatic language skills in controls (F [1, 53] = 4.20, p = .046, η 2 p = .08 [90% CI = 0–.20]), whereas the time dwelling on the eyes was not significantly associated with pragmatic ability among the women with the FMR1 premutation (F [1, 53] = 0.79, p = .378, η 2 p = .02 [90% CI = 0–.11]). For both general linear regression models, two observations exceeded recommended cut-off criteria for influence for Cook’s D (i.e., Di > 4/n-k-1; [61]); however, inference across solutions including and excluding the observations were identical and thus only the solution associated with complete data is presented here for parsimony. Regression coefficients are presented in Table 3.

Table 3 Regression coefficients depicting dwelling on the eyes as a predictor of pragmatic language ability

Discussion

The FMR1 premutation is a prevalent genetic mutation associated with a range of cognitive, affective, and physical health complications including pragmatic language deficits that are of clinical significance. The present study represents the first investigation of attention to eye gaze in women with the FMR1 premutation, with the hypothesis that this fundamental social-cognitive skill may be impaired in this group and may underlie pragmatic language features. Findings showed increased salience of averted gaze among the women with the FMR1 premutation relative to control women. Additionally, group-specific patterns were detected where greater attention to eye gaze was associated with enhanced pragmatic language competence in control women, but not in women with the FMR1 premutation. The decoupling of gaze sensitivity and pragmatic language ability in the FMR1 premutation provides preliminary evidence that the development of social communication skills differs between carriers of the FMR1 premutation and controls. This study informs the interface between component social-cognitive and language features of the FMR1 premutation phenotype and highlights future avenues of research aimed at identifying causal mechanisms.

Increased salience of averted gaze in the FMR1 premutation

Study controls dwelled longer on the eyes during the direct gaze condition compared to averted gaze, which is consistent with evidence supporting a human preference for direct gaze [2325]. Our results showed that women with the FMR1 premutation failed to demonstrate this preferential attention to direct gaze, suggesting that the perceived significance of eye gaze may be aberrant in this group. Notably, the atypical attentional patterns in the premutation group appear to be driven by increased saliency of averted gaze rather than diminished saliency of direct gaze, as time spent dwelling on the eyes was increased relative to controls only in response to the averted condition. Why, then, did the women with the FMR1 premutation dwell longer on the eyes only in response to averted gaze? The eyes convey significant social information and increased attention to the eyes may be interpreted as an effort to compensate for inefficient processing of social input. Averted gaze, which has more ambiguous meaning than direct gaze, may have required greater effort to process than direct gaze (and hence was associated with longer dwell times). Another possibility is that the women with the FMR1 premutation simply failed to recognize direct gaze as having increase saliency relative to averted gaze, which would suggest a disruption in innate, biologically-driven recognition of core social cues. Atypical processing of other fundamental social signals has been documented in women with the FMR1 premutation, such as reduced sensitivity to biological motion (e.g., [13]). This interpretation is also consistent with neuroimaging studies of the FMR1 premutation documenting reduced activation of the amygdala and superior temporal sulcus [9, 10], which are brain areas implicated in gaze processing and social cognition [62].

It seems most likely that the looking behavior of the women in this study reflected social-cognitive processes, given the large body of research supports the fundamental role of social gaze in the development and exchange of social-cognitive knowledge [2022, 2633]. However, we cannot be sure what underlying processes motivated the looking behavior of the women, and it is possible that other factors influenced attention to the eyes. One alternative explanation is that elevated social anxiety drove the attentional vigilance towards averted gaze, as elevated rates of social anxiety have been documented among women with the FMR1 premutation [63, 64]. Social anxiety is a condition characterized by fear of negative social evaluation and attentional bias for social-evaluative threat cues, and one function of averted gaze is to convey negative relational evaluation and social exclusion [65, 66]. A prior study employing these stimuli in a similar eye-tracking paradigm found that female college students with high levels of social anxiety showed higher mean fixation duration on the eyes than those with low social anxiety, although group differences did not reach statistical significance [54]. It is also possible that executive deficits contributed to the atypical dwelling patterns of the women with the FMR1 premutation. Women with the FMR1 premutation show subtle deficits across a range of executive processes [6769], and executive control would be required to shift attention from the eyes to other parts of the face. Follow-up investigations are needed to clarify the underlying processes responsible for altered sensitivity to social gaze in the FMR1 premutation.

Relationship between eye gaze sensitivity and pragmatic language across groups

Contrary to hypotheses, attention to eye gaze was not associated with pragmatic language ability in women with the FMR1 premutation. Yet, these variables were linked among study controls; control women who attended more carefully to the eyes showed enhanced pragmatic language competence. This finding in controls is consistent with social-pragmatic theory and a large body of research showing that the eyes convey essential social-cognitive information that facilitates social-communicative engagement [38, 39]. Thus, the lack of relationship between gaze processing and pragmatics in the FMR1 premutation suggests that attending to the eyes did not translate to enhanced registration of social cognitive information in this group. A critical question remains: if attention to social gaze did not correlate with pragmatic language ability in the FMR1 premutation, what does? Better understanding of the factors associated with pragmatic language difficulties is critical to the development of family-centered prevention/intervention programs that can best support the complex needs of families affected by fragile X-associated conditions.

Implications for theory, research, and practice

Findings showed a relationship between attention to eye gaze and social-communicative ability in control women that was of a medium-to-large effect. In some respects, it is surprising that the time spent dwelling on the eyes of an animated face in a controlled experimental context was strongly associated with pragmatic performance sampled from an independent, semi-naturalistic communicative interaction. The strength of the association between these behaviors, despite the disparate contexts from which they were measured, underscores the importance of the eyes in navigating social interactions. Although a wealth of empirical studies have established sensitivity to eye gaze as foundational skill for social-communicative engagement, the majority of this work has focused on early development and relatively little is known about the functions and use of eye gaze in adult populations. This study sheds light on social-cognitive correlates of communicative behavior in adults and suggests that the application of social-pragmatic theory is not limited to childhood. While this study examined subtle pragmatic language variation in non-disordered individuals, findings may also have relevance for disordered populations, where the prevalence of adults with social-communication impairments, such as those with autism spectrum disorder, is increasing (e.g., [70]). The importance of understanding the nature and basis of pragmatic language impairment is underscored by the recent addition of social communication disorder as its own diagnostic category in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders, which is used by clinicians, researchers, and public health officials to classify mental health disorders (DSM-5; [71]). This diagnostic shift recognizes the functional limitations pragmatic language impairments can pose on social participation, relationships, and academic/occupational performance.

This study is the first to examine correlates of pragmatic language difficulty in the FMR1 premutation. Research along these lines may have implications for clinical practice. Although pragmatic deficits of the FMR1 premutation are generally considered to be subclinical and mild in nature, the presence of these features has been shown to impact outcomes at the family level [14]. Furthermore, pragmatic language difficulties comprise a principal feature of autism and the broad autism phenotype, both of which are elevated in women with the FMR1 premutation [11]. Clinician knowledge of features that interface with social-communication variation in FMR1 conditions can be helpful in supporting effective clinical interactions. Successful social-communicative interactions are arguably at the crux of the human condition, as this skill supports an innate human need for social relationships with others [72]. Strong interpersonal skills are associated with a number of life advantages, such as enhanced relationship satisfaction, increased social support, and reduced stress and psychological symptoms [72]. This study sheds light on attention to eye gaze as a powerful social tool that is associated with social-communicative engagement in the general population, and which appears to be aberrant in the FMR1 premutation. Future research may explore eye gaze processing as a potential marker that may relate to other features of the FMR1 premutation clinical phenotype, potentially contributing to the identification of vulnerable subgroups.

Limitations and future directions

This study represents a first attempt to characterize sensitivity to eye gaze in women with the FMR1 premutation, and number of questions remain open to investigation. First, it is difficult to determine causal relationships with certainty, given the correlational design. In developmental research and theory, the ability to process social gaze is considered a foundation for the later development of social-communication skills, and hence it was assumed that the associations examined here would follow the same direction. However, other directional relationships seem plausible, such as the case where an individual who is aware of their own pragmatic difficulties may learn to compensate by attending more carefully to social signals. We were also unable to sample sensitivity to eye gaze occurring directly during the social-communicative interaction, instead relying on animated simulation of eye contact. While the relationships observed in study controls supports the utility of the stimuli in tapping the intended social-cognitive constructs, the use of more naturalistic stimuli may have yielded different results (e.g., [73]). Future studies might take advantage of new technologies, such as wearable eye-tracking devices, which would allow for the quantification of eye gaze patterns during dynamic social interaction. It should also be noted that the sample was primarily Caucasian and limited to mothers who had a child identified as having fragile X syndrome or the FMR1 premutation, which may limit generalizability. Finally, there are some limitations associated with relying on non-significant p values to establish group equivalency [74, 75]; while the groups in this study did not differ significantly on relevant demographic variables, the use of more rigorous matching criteria would have provided further support of group equivalency.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this study contributes to the growing characterization of social phenotypes associated with the FMR1 premutation by documenting altered sensitivity to the eyes in the FMR1 premutation, characterized by increased salience of averted gaze. Eye gaze is a critical medium through which social information is conveyed, supporting effective communication. Our findings suggest that, unlike study controls, women with the FMR1 premutation were unable to capitalize on information conveyed through the eyes to enhance social-communicative engagement. This study sheds light on mechanisms associated with pragmatic language variation in adults, and how they may be impaired in specific ways in the FMR1 premutation. The characterization of altered gaze processing in FMR1 premutation during earlier developmental periods constitutes an important area of future investigation, given the implications of intervening with this high-risk group during developmentally sensitive periods. Efforts to define phenotypes and mechanisms associated with the FMR1 premutation have significant public health implications, given the high prevalence of this genetic condition and its relatively undefined clinical profile.

Abbreviations

FMR1 :

Fragile X Mental Retardation-1

FXTAS:

Fragile X-associated tremor ataxia syndrome

References

  1. Hunter J, Rivero-Arias O, Angelov A, Kim E, Fotheringham I, Leal J. Epidemiology of fragile X syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Med Genet A. 2014;164:1648–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Crawford DC, Acuna JM, Sherman SL. FMR1 and the fragile X syndrome: human genome epidemiology review. Genet Med. 2001;3:359–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Nolin SL, Brown WT, Glicksman A, Houck JGE, Gargano AD, Sullivan A, Biancalana V, Bröndum-Nielsen K, Hjalgrim H, Holinski-Feder E, et al. Expansion of the fragile X CGG repeat in females with premutation or intermediate alleles. Am J Hum Genet. 2003;72:454–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Roberts JE, Tonnsen BL, McCary LM, Ford AL, Golden RN, Bailey DB. Trajectory and predictors of depression and anxiety disorders in mothers with the FMR1 Premutation. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79:850–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ, Wong LM, McLennan Y, Srivastava S, Tassone F, Harvey D, Rivera SM, Simon TJ. Young adult female fragile X premutation carriers show age- and genetically-modulated cognitive impairments. Brain Cogn. 2011;75(3):255–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Rodriguez-Revenga L, Madrigal I, Pagonabarraga J, Xuncla M, Badenas C, Kulisevsky J, Gomez B, Mila M. Penetrance of FMR1 premutation associated pathologies in fragile X syndrome families. Eur J Hum Genet. 2009;17:1359–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Sullivan AK, Marcus M, Epstein MP, Allen EG, Anido AE, Paquin JJ, Yadav-Shah M, Sherman SL. Association of FMR1 repeat size with ovarian dysfunction. Hum Reprod. 2005;20:402–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Cornish K, Kogan C, Turk J, Manly T, James N, Mills A, Dalton A. The emerging fragile X premuation phenotype: evidence from the domain of social cognition. Brain Cogn. 2005;57:53–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hessl D, Wang JM, Schneider A, Koldewyn K, Le L, Iwahashi C, Cheung K, Tassone F, Hagerman PJ, Rivera SM. Decreased FMRP expression underlies amygdala dysfunction in carriers of the fragile X premutation. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:859–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Hessl D, Rivera S, Koldewyn K, Cordeiro L, Adams J, Tassone F, Hagerman PJ, Hagerman RJ. Amygdala dysfunction in men with the fragile X premutation. Brain. 2007;130:404–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Clifford S, Dissanayake C, Bui QM, Huggins R, Taylor AK, Loesch DZ. Autism spectrum phenotype in males and females with fragile X full mutation and premutation. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:738–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Losh M, Klusek J, Martin GE, Sideris J, Parlier M, Piven J. Defining genetically meaningful language and personality traits in relatives of individuals with fragile X syndrome and relatives of individuals with autism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2012;159B:660–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Keri S, Benedek G. The perception of biological and mechanical motion in female fragile X premutation carriers. Brain Cogn. 2010;72:197–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Klusek J, McGrath SE, Abbeduto L, Roberts JE. Pragmatic language features of mothers with the FMR1 premutation are associated with the language outcomes of adolescents and young adults with fragile X syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2016;59:49–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Whitehouse A, Watt H, Line E, Bishop D. Adult psychosocial outcomes of children with specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and autism. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2009;44:511–28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Laws G, Bates G, Feuerstein M, Mason-Apps E, White C. Peer acceptance of children with language and communication impairments in a mainstream primary school: associations with type of language difficulty, problem behaviours and a change in placement organization. Child Lang Teach Ther. 2012;28:73–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Mackie L, Law J. Pragmatic language and the child with emotional/behavioral difficulties (EBD): a pilot study exploring the interaction between behaviour and communication disability. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2010;45:397–410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Jobe LE, White SW. Loneliness, social relationships, and a broader autism phenotype in college students. Personal Individ Differ. 2007;42:1479–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Nation K, Penny S. Sensitivity to eye gaze in autism: is it normal? Is it automatic? Is it social? Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20:79–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Farroni T, Csibra G, Simion F, Johnson MH. Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2002;99:9602–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Grossmann T, Johnson MH, Farroni T, Csibra G. Social perception in the infant brain: gamma oscillatory activity in response to eye gaze. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2:284–91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Baron-Cohen S, Cross P. Reading the eyes: evidence for the role of perception in the development of a theory of mind. Mind Lang. 1992;7:172–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Conty L, Tijus C, Hugueville L, Coelho E, George N. Searching for asymmetries in the detection of gaze contact versus averted gaze under different head views: a behavioural study. Spat Vis. 2006;19:529–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Senju A, Hasegawa T, Tojo Y. Does perceived direct gaze boost detection in adults and children with and without autism? The stare-in-the-crowd effect revisited. Vis Cogn. 2005;12:1474–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Senju A, Hasegawa T. Direct gaze captures visuospatial attention. Vis Cogn. 2005;12:127–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Baron-Cohen S, Ring H: A model of the mindreading system: neuropsychological and neurobiological perspectives. Origins of an understanding of mind 1994:183-207.

  27. Guellai B, Streri A. Cues for early social skills: direct gaze modulates newborns’ recognition of talking faces. PLoS One. 2011;6:e18610.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Macrae CN, Hood BM, Milne AB, Rowe AC, Mason MF. Are you looking at me? Eye gaze and person perception. Psychol Sci. 2002;13:460–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Hood BM, Macrae CN, Cole-Davies V, Dias M. Eye remember you: the effects of gaze direction on face recognition in children and adults. Dev Sci. 2003;6:67–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Farroni T, Massaccesi S, Menon E, Johnson MH. Direct gaze modulates face recognition in young infants. Cognition. 2007;102:396–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Milders M, Hietanen JK, Leppänen JM, Braun M. Detection of emotional faces is modulated by the direction of eye gaze. Emotion. 2011;11:1456–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Adams RB, Kleck RE. Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:644–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Adams RB, Kleck RE. Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion. 2005;5:3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Brooks R, Meltzoff AN. The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Dev Sci. 2005;8:535–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Morales M, Mundy P, Delgado CE, Yale M, Messinger D, Neal R, Schwartz HK. Responding to joint attention across the 6-through 24-month age period and early language acquisition. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2000;21:283–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Baron-Cohen S, Baldwin DA, Crowson M. Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Dev. 1997;68:48–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Brooks R, Meltzoff AN. Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: a longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. J Child Lang. 2008;35:207–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Tomasello M, Tomasello M. Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard university press; 2009.

  39. Bruner J: The acquisition of pragmatic commitments. The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication 1983:27-42.

  40. Baron-Cohen S. Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Frith U, Frith C. The biological basis of social interaction. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2001;10:151–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Frischen A, Bayliss AP, Tipper SP. Gaze cueing of attention: visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:694.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Kleinke CL. Gaze and eye contact: a research review. Psychol Bull. 1986;100:78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Langton SR, Bruce V. Reflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of others. Vis Cogn. 1999;6:541–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Hanna JE, Brennan SE. Speakers’ eye gaze disambiguates referring expressions early during face-to-face conversation. J Mem Lang. 2007;57:596–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. McCarthy A, Lee K. Children’s knowledge of deceptive gaze cues and its relation to their actual lying behavior. J Exp Child Psychol. 2009;103:117–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Richmond VP, McCroskey JC, Payne SK. Nonverbal behavior in interpersonal relations. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall; 1991.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Ho S, Foulsham T, Kingstone A. Speaking and listening with the eyes: gaze signaling during dyadic interactions. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0136905.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Vertegaal R, Slagter R, Van der Veer G, Nijholt A: Eye gaze patterns in conversations: there is more to conversational agents than meets the eyes. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human factors in computing systems. Seattle: ACM; 2001: 301–8.

  50. Klusek J, LaFauci G, Adayev T, Brown WT, Tassone F, Roberts JE. Reduced vagal tone in women with the FMR1 premutation is associated with FMR1 mRNA but not depression or anxiety. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2017;9:1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. 2nd ed. Pearson Assessments: Los Angeles; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Louis ED, Barnes LF, Wendt KJ, Albert SM, Pullman SL, Yu Q, Schneier FR. Validity and test-retest reliability of a disability questionnaire for essential tremor. Mov Disord. 2000;15:516–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Rutter M, Bailey A, Lord C. SCQ: the social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Wieser MJ, Pauli P, Alpers GW, Mühlberger A. Is eye to eye contact really threatening and avoided in social anxiety?—an eye-tracking and psychophysiology study. J Anxiety Disord. 2009;23:93–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Landa R, Piven J, Wzorek MM, Gayle JO, Chase GA, Folstein SE. Social language use in parents of autistic individuals. Psychol Med. 1992;22:245–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Klusek J, Losh M, Martin G. Sex differences and within-family associations in the broad autism phenotype. Autism. 2014;18:106–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Cicchetti DV. Methodological commentary the precision of reliability and validity estimates re-visited: distinguishing between clinical and statistical significance of sample size requirements. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2001;23:695–700.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. SAS Institute: SAS Institute version 9.4. Cary NC; 2013.

  59. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol. 1995;57:289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. L. Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Cook RD. Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics. 1977:15–8.

  62. Itier RJ, Batty M. Neural bases of eye and gaze processing: the core of social cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:843–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Franke P, Leboyer M, Gansicke M, Weiffenbacj O. Genotype-phenotype relationship in female carriers of the premutation and full mutation of FMR-1. Psychiatry Res. 1998;90:113–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Bourgeois JA, Seritan AL, Casillas EM, Hessl D, Schneider A, Yang Y, Kaur I, Cogswell JB, Nguyen DV, Hagerman RJ. Lifetime prevalence of mood and anxiety disorders in fragile X premutation carriers. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72:175–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Wirth JH, Sacco DF, Hugenberg K, Williams KD. Eye gaze as relational evaluation: averted eye gaze leads to feelings of ostracism and relational devaluation. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2010;36:869–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Mogg K, Philippot P, Bradley BP. Selective attention to angry faces in clinical social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2004;113:160–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Shelton AL, Cornish K, Kraan C, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Metcalfe SA, Bradshaw JL, Hocking DR, Archibald AD, Cohen J, Trollor JN. Exploring inhibitory deficits in female premutation carriers of fragile X syndrome: through eye movements. Brain Cogn. 2014;85:201–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Kraan CM, Hocking DR, Bradshaw JL, Fielding J, Cohen J, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Cornish KM. Neurobehavioural evidence for the involvement of the FMR1 gene in female carriers of fragile X syndrome. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:522–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Shelton AL, Cornish KM, Kraan CM, Lozano R, Bui M, Fielding J. Executive dysfunction in female FMR1 premutation carriers. Cerebellum. 2016;15:565–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Baxter AJ, Brugha TS, Erskine HE, Scheurer RW, Vos T, Scott JG. The epidemiology and global burden of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol Med. 2015;45:601–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed.; DSM-5 edn: American Psychiatric Publishing, Incorporated; 2013.

  72. Hargie O. Skilled interpersonal communication: research, theory and practice. New York: Routledge; 2010.

  73. Risko EF, Richardson DC, Kingstone A. Breaking the fourth wall of cognitive science: real-world social attention and the dual function of gaze. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016;25:70–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Mervis CB, Klein-Tasman BP. Methodological issues in group-matching designs: α levels for control variable comparisons and measurement characteristics of control and target variables. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:7–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Kover ST, Atwood AK. Establishing equivalence: methodological progress in group-matching design and analysis. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2013;118:3–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the women who participated in this study.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health (F32DC013934, PI: Klusek; R01MH090194, PI: Roberts; R01HD024356, PI: Abbeduto), the Research Participant Registry Core of the Carolina Institute for Developmental Disabilities (P30HD03110), and the IDDRC Administrative Core (U54HD079125).

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JK conceived the study and lead the data collection, analysis, and interpretation. JER provided guidance on study design, data collection, and interpretation. JS programmed the eye gaze paradigm task, oversaw data collection of the eye gaze data, and assisted with interpretation and presentation of the eye-tracking data. AP assisted with data collection and management. AF provided the guidance on the statistical approach. JK drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the results and critical revision the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jessica Klusek.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all participants. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of South Carolina.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Klusek, J., Schmidt, J., Fairchild, A.J. et al. Altered sensitivity to social gaze in the FMR1 premutation and pragmatic language competence. J Neurodevelop Disord 9, 31 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-017-9211-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-017-9211-z

Keywords