Skip to main content

Associations between sensory processing and electrophysiological and neurochemical measures in children with ASD: an EEG-MRS study

Abstract

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is associated with hyper- and/or hypo-sensitivity to sensory input. Spontaneous alpha power, which plays an important role in shaping responsivity to sensory information, is reduced across the lifespan in individuals with ASD. Furthermore, an excitatory/inhibitory imbalance has also been linked to sensory dysfunction in ASD and has been hypothesized to underlie atypical patterns of spontaneous brain activity. The present study examined whether resting-state alpha power differed in children with ASD as compared to TD children, and investigated the relationships between alpha levels, concentrations of excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters, and atypical sensory processing in ASD.

Methods

Participants included thirty-one children and adolescents with ASD and thirty-one age- and IQ-matched typically developing (TD) participants. Resting-state electroencephalography (EEG) was used to obtain measures of alpha power. A subset of participants (ASD = 16; TD = 16) also completed a magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) protocol in order to measure concentrations of excitatory (glutamate + glutamine; Glx) and inhibitory (GABA) neurotransmitters.

Results

Children with ASD evidenced significantly decreased resting alpha power compared to their TD peers. MRS estimates of GABA and Glx did not differ between groups with the exception of Glx in the temporal-parietal junction. Inter-individual differences in alpha power within the ASD group were not associated with region-specific concentrations of GABA or Glx, nor were they associated with sensory processing differences. However, atypically decreased Glx was associated with increased sensory impairment in children with ASD.

Conclusions

Although we replicated prior reports of decreased alpha power in ASD, atypically reduced alpha was not related to neurochemical differences or sensory symptoms in ASD. Instead, reduced Glx in the temporal-parietal cortex was associated with greater hyper-sensitivity in ASD. Together, these findings may provide insight into the neural underpinnings of sensory processing differences present in ASD.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is an etiologically complex, heterogeneous condition affecting 1 in 54 children, making it one of the most prevalent neurodevelopmental disorders [1]. ASD is a behaviorally defined disorder that is diagnosed on the basis of impairments in social communication and the presence of restricted and repetitive behaviors, including atypical responsivity to sensory stimuli [2]. These differences include hyper-, hypo-, and a mixed pattern of hypo- and hyper-sensitivity to sensory input [3]. Importantly, prior research has shown atypical sensory responsivity is present within the first year of life in high-risk infants later diagnosed with ASD [4], and is associated with other core ASD symptoms, including sociocommunicative impairments [5] and restricted and repetitive behaviors [4]. Thus, insight into the source(s) of sensory processing differences may also assist in the explaining the emergence of the heterogeneous ASD phenotype.

Behavioral responses to incoming sensory information are determined, in part, by one’s cortical state [6]. Electroencephalogram (EEG) is a powerful tool for studying spontaneous brain activation associated with patterns of cortical synaptic activity. Neural oscillations, as measured by EEG, play a key role in brain function and are associated with a variety of perceptual and cognitive processes [7]. In particular, the alpha band (8–12 Hz) has been linked to attentional and perceptual processing [8, 9]. For example, pre-stimulus (i.e., spontaneous) alpha power is associated with the detection of briefly presented visual and tactile stimuli [10,11,12]. In light of these and other findings, several theories have outlined how alpha activity may play an active role in modulating sensory input [9, 13, 14]. In particular, Jensen and Mazaheri [13] and Mathewson and colleagues [14] have proposed that alpha oscillations may function as a sensory gating mechanism through pulsed inhibition, which is mediated by activity of GABAergic inhibitory interneurons. Together, these theories suggest that ongoing alpha oscillatory activity plays a critical role in shaping perception of and responses to incoming sensory information.

Previous studies have focused on neurophysiological differences in ASD, and how they may contribute to the behavioral characteristics associated with the disorder (see [15], for review). Specifically within the alpha band, reductions in alpha are present as early as 3 [16] to 6 months [17] in infants at high risk for ASD. Further, significantly reduced alpha power has also been shown in school-aged children and adolescents [18,19,20,21] as well as adults [22] with ASD (although see [23,24,25], for evidence of greater, or, [26], for evidence of equivalent alpha power in ASD). Within ASD, differences in alpha power have been related to sensory hypo-responsiveness [27], sensory seeking behavior [28], and greater attention to detail [24], suggesting that they may contribute to the sensory processing differences present in individuals with ASD (see [29], for review).

Based, in part, on evidence of reduced alpha power in ASD, Wang and colleagues [15] suggested that individuals with ASD may display a U-shaped profile of EEG power differences with greater low- (delta, theta) and high- (beta, gamma) and reduced mid-frequency (alpha) power in ASD. They hypothesize that this pattern of ASD-related power differences may reflect atypical patterns of excitatory (glutamate) and/or inhibitory (GABA; E/I) neurotransmitters, which is consistent with models that have proposed that ASD may result from atypically increased cortical excitation (Hussman [30]; Rubenstein and Merzenich [31]). Although in vivo measurement of GABA and glutamate concentrations using magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) has provided mixed results (see [32], for review), region-specific differences in GABA concentrations in ASD have been linked to sensory processing differences [33,34,35]. However, it is currently unclear whether differences in regional patterns of neurotransmitter concentrations are associated with atypical oscillary activity in ASD, and whether inter-individual differences in alpha power and E/I measures may be related to hyper- and/or hypo-sensory sensitivity in ASD.

The current study examines whether differences in alpha-band power are present in children and adolescents with ASD compared to their TD peers and investigates whether a relationship exists between alpha and measures of ASD symptomatology, specifically sensory processing differences. We hypothesize that children with ASD will exhibit reduced alpha levels compared to their TD peers and that decreased alpha power will be associated with increased sensory processing symptoms in ASD. Furthermore, based on the theoretical and empirical links between excitatory/inhibitory (im)balance and atypical neural oscillations and sensory function in ASD, we also sought to examine the associations between in vivo measures of excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters, alpha power, and sensory symptoms. By comparing not only the physiological features of both groups, but mapping those onto measures of ASD symptomatology, findings from the current study may provide a better understanding about the heterogeneous nature of ASD symptoms and their neurophysiological bases.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 31 children with ASD and 31 age- and non-verbal IQ-matched TD children (see Table 1). IQ scores were determined based on the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Second Edition (WASI-II [36]) or the Differential Abilities Scales, Second Edition (DAS-2 [37]). Clinical diagnoses were confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2 [38]), Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ [39]), and expert clinical judgment according to DSM-5 criteria [2]. Participants in the ASD group were excluded if they had any known non-idiopathic forms of ASD, such as Fragile X syndrome. Children with non-verbal IQ scores < 70 were excluded. Typically developing children and adolescents had no significant ASD symptomatology or family history of ASD as confirmed via parent report.

Table 1 Participant characteristics

Electroencephalography (EEG)

EEG acquisition

Participants were instructed to relax, remain as still as possible, and look ahead at a black fixation cross on a gray background. EEG data were recorded for 3, 2-min blocks in this eyes-open resting state. EEG was acquired using 128-channel high-density Geodesic sensor nets (Electrical Geodesics, Inc.; Eugene, OR) with a NetAmps 400 high-input amplifier. Data were collected from 124 of 128 possible channel locations. In order to decrease attrition, EOG electrodes (electrodes placed on the face) were not used. Data were sampled at 500 Hz and referenced to the vertex electrode.

EEG analysis

EEG data were processed using MATLAB-based toolbox EEGLAB [40]. Data were filtered (1–50 Hz), bad channels were removed, non-stereotyped artifacts were manually rejected, and then independent component analysis (ICA) completed. Next, SemiAutomatic Selection of Independent Components for Artifact correction in the EEG (SASICA [41]) was used to identify artifacts associated with blinks, saccades, muscle contractions, and bad channels [41]. After removing EEG activity associated with artifactual components, bad channels were replaced using spherical interpolation, and data were re-referenced to the average reference. Groups did not differ significantly in the number of bad channels replaced (ASD: 2 [6]; TD: 1 [2]), t(60) = 1.4, p = .156, or components removed (ASD: 9 [3]; TD: 8 [3]), t(60) = 1.4, p = .168. Artifact-corrected data were segmented into 1-s epochs, and epochs containing residual artifacts were rejected. Alpha power (8–12 Hz), expressed as decibels (dB), was extracted from three regions of interest (ROI) each of which consisted of 4 locations across midline frontal (Fz; 4, 11, 16, 19), central (Cz; 7, 55, 106, 129), and posterior electrode locations (Pz; 62, 67, 72, 77; see Fig. 1) using the EEGLAB function spectopo, which computes power spectral density with the frequency resolution of 0.25 Hz.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Scalp maps of resting-state alpha (8–12 Hz) power for the autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing (TD) groups. Regions of interest (ROIs) examined are displayed as black dots. b Mean alpha power (db) for ASD (gray) and TD (white) for each ROI. Error bars represent ± 1 SEM. *p < .05

Magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS)

A subset of age- and IQ-matched participants (ASD = 16; TD = 16) completed the imaging protocol (see Supplementary Table 1). These data have previously been reported in Edmondson et al. [42]. MRI and MRS data were acquired using a 3-T Prisma Siemens scanner with a 64-channel head coil. To minimize movement, participants’ heads were stabilized with foam padding and they were instructed to remain as still as possible and watch a video of their choice for the duration of the scan. A high-resolution T1-weighted image (MPRAGE, TE = 4.91, TR = 2000, TI = 977, FA = 9) was taken for MRS voxel placement and tissue segmentation. Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) was acquired using MEGA-semi-LASER localization (TE = 68, TR = 2000, averages = 128, acquisition time = 8:56) [43]. To measure Glx (glutamate + glutamine) 1H-MR spectroscopy was acquired using semi-LASER localization (TE = 35, TR = 2000, averages = 64, acquisition time = 2:28) [44]. Unsuppressed water acquisitions were acquired as reference scans both for phase and eddy-current corrections as well as for quantification (ratio of metabolite over water) for both semi-LASER and MEGA-sLASER. Parameters for the reference scans were the same as for water suppressed scans with the exception of only 8 averages acquired. Total acquisition time was approximately 45 min.

Volumes of interest (VOI) were placed in the right frontal eye fields (rFEF, 20 × 30 × 2), right temporal-parietal junction (rTPJ, 30 × 20 × 30), and visual cortex (VIS, 30 × 30 × 20) using anatomical landmarks (see [42], for more details; Fig. 2). Absolute quantification of GABA and Glx was performed on spectra from each VOI with LCModel V6.3-1B [45]. Basis sets used in LCModel were generated using density matrix simulation and using GABA coupling constants from Kaiser et al. [46]. Results from LCModel for each neurochemical were in mM and only neurochemicals with Cramer-Rao lower bounds (CRLB) of < 20% across all participants were used for subsequent analyses. Tissue segmentation to obtain percentages of white matter, gray matter, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) was performed using SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). All metabolites were CSF-corrected with the exception of GABA, which was tissue-corrected using the method as described in Harris et al. [47] using an α = 0.5. Finally, MRS GABA+ and Glx values were log-transformed.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Volumes of interest (VOI) for right frontal eye fields (rFEF; left column), right temporal-parietal junction (rTPJ; center column), and visual cortex (VIS; right column) and non-transformed GABA+ and Glx values for ASD (gray) and TD (white) groups. Error bars represent ± 1 SEM. *p < .05

Measures

Sensory Profile-2 (SP-2 [48])

The SP-2 is an 86-item caregiver-report questionnaire for 3- to 14-year-olds that measures four sensory processing categories: Seeking, Avoiding, Sensitivity, and Registration. Caregivers respond to each item using a five-point Likert scale with a score of 1 indicating a behavior that is present almost never and 5 indicating a behavior that is present almost always. The Seeking and Registration categories are related to hypo-sensitivity or high sensory thresholds, with Seeking being characteristic of a child that obtains sensory input and Registration being characteristic of a child that misses sensory input. The Sensitivity and Avoiding categories are related to hyper-sensitivity or low sensory thresholds, with Sensitivity associated with the degree that a child detects sensory input and Avoiding associated with the degree a child is bothered by sensory input. Higher scores in each quadrant are indicative of greater sensory symptoms.

Results

Alpha power

Absolute alpha power was entered into a 2 (group: ASD, TD) × 3 (ROI: frontal, central, posterior) mixed-model repeated measures ANOVA. There was a significant effect of ROI, F(2, 120) = 122.4, p < .001, ηp2 = 0.67, as power was greater at the posterior compared to both frontal, t(61) = − 9.6, p < .001, and central, t(61) = − 13.5, p < .001, ROI, and greater at the frontal compared to central ROI, t(61) = 3.8, p < .001. Children with ASD showed marginally lower alpha power compared with TD children, F(1, 60) = 3.3, p = .075, ηp2 = 0.05. Additionally, as illustrated in Fig. 1, there was a significant interaction between group and ROI, F(2, 120) = 5.0, p = .008, ηp2 = 0.08. Follow-up t tests revealed significant group differences at the posterior ROI, t(60) = − 2.3, p = .024, but not the central, t(60) = − 1.4, p = .169, or frontal, t(60) = − 1.0, p = .307, ROIs. The percentage of epochs rejected did differ significantly between ASD (M = 33%; SD = 16%) and TD (M = 18%; SD = 15%) groups, t(60) = 3.7, p < .001; however, the percentage of rejected epochs was not associated with average alpha power across groups, r(60) = − .080, p = .536, and the pattern of results was unchanged when the percentage of rejected epochs was entered as a covariate in the ANOVA.

Lastly, a separate set of analyses was conducted to identify individual alpha frequency (IAF) using the method outlined by Corcoran and colleagues [49] and determine absolute low- (IAF-2Hz to IAF), high- (IAF to IAF+2Hz), and combined-alpha (IAF-2Hz to IAF+2Hz) power according to IAF. There were no differences in IAF between groups (ASD = 9.441 Hz; TD = 9.438 Hz, p = .99). Furthermore, the results for IAF-alpha power analyses were equivalent to our original analysis (i.e., significant interaction between group and ROI; significant difference at posterior ROI between ASD and TD).

MRS

GABA+ and Glx values were entered into separate mixed-model repeated measures ANOVA with between-subjects factor group (ASD, TD) and within-subjects factor VOI (rFEF, rTPJ, and VIS). As previously reported in Edmondson et al. [42], while GABA+ levels varied by VOI, F(2, 54) = 152.7, p < .001, ηp2 = 0.85, there were no significant between group differences, F(1, 27) = 0.01, p = .943, ηp2 = 0.00, nor was there an interaction between group and VOI, F(2, 54) = 0.89, p = .416, ηp2 = 0.03. Similar to GABA+, Glx varied significantly across VOI, F(2, 60) = 22.3, p < .001, ηp2 = 0.43. However, in contrast to GABA+, Glx values were significantly lower in ASD, F(1, 30) = 9.2, p = .005, ηp2 = 0.24. The significant main effect of group was subsumed by a significant group by VOI interaction, F(2, 60) = 6.00, p = .004, ηp2 = 0.17. Follow-up t tests showed that the ASD group had significantly reduced Glx in the rTPJ, t(30) = − 3.2, p = .003, but not the rFEF, t(30) = − 1.3, p = .194, or VIS, t(30) = − 1.2, p = .233, VOI (see Fig. 2).

Correlational analyses

GABA+, Glx, and alpha power

For all participants, increased VIS GABA+ was associated with greater frontal alpha power, r(60) = .408, p = .021. Within the TD group, VIS GABA+ values were significantly correlated with frontal alpha power, r(14) = .500, p = .048; however, these were not significantly correlated in the ASD group, r(14) = .362, p = .169. There were no other significant association between GABA+ values and alpha power across all participants or within ASD or TD groups. Additionally, there were no significant correlations between alpha power and Glx values across all participants, or within ASD and TD groups (all p values > .05; see Supplementary Table 2).

GABA+, Glx, alpha, and sensory processing

On the SP-2, the ASD and TD groups differed significantly on all quadrants (see Table 1). Across both groups, no significant correlations were found between SP-2 scores and alpha levels for any ROI (all p > .13). However, for the TD group greater central alpha power was associated with increased Sensitivity, r(29) = .398, p = .026, and Seeking, r(29) = .408, p = .023, scores. There were no significant associations between SP-2 scores and alpha power within the ASD group (all p > .49; see Supplementary Table 3).

For the MRS values, there was a significant association between rTPJ Glx values and Registration, r(30) = − .406, p = .021; Sensitivity, r(30) = − .581, p < .001; Avoiding, r(30) = − .619, p < .001; and Seeking, r(30) = − .447, p = .010, scores across all participants. These correlations were due, in part, to correlations within the ASD group, particularly for Sensitivity, r(14) = − .493, p = .052, and for Avoidance, r(14) = − .502, p = .048, scores (see Fig. 3). No significant correlations between MRS and SP-2 measures were present within the TD group (all p > .20; see Supplementary Table 4).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Scatterplots displaying associations between right temporal-parietal junction (rTPJ) Glx values and Avoiding and Sensitivity quadrant scores from the Sensory Profile-2 (SP-2)

Discussion

The present study sought to examine differences in resting-state alpha power between children with ASD and TD children as well as the associations between alpha levels, concentrations of excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters, and atypical sensory processing. To our knowledge this is the first study to examine whether atypical patterns of spontaneous brain activity, as measured by EEG, are associated with neurochemical differences in ASD. Consistent with a large body of previous research, children with ASD showed significantly increased sensory symptoms and reduced alpha power compared to their TD peers. However, inter-individual differences in alpha power within the ASD group were not associated with sensory processing differences, nor were they associated with region-specific concentrations of GABA or Glx. Children with ASD did show reduced Glx values in the right temporal-parietal junction compared to their TD peers, and atypically, decreased Glx was associated with elevated sensory processing symptoms in children with ASD. Together, these findings may provide insight into the neural underpinnings of sensory processing differences present in ASD.

Alpha, excitation/inhibition, and sensory symptoms in ASD

In agreement with previous reports from infancy to adulthood (e.g., [16, 22]), we found reduced spontaneous absolute alpha power in children with ASD. Based on this atypical pattern of band-specific activity in ASD, Wang and colleagues [15] proposed that reduced mid-frequency (i.e., alpha) power may be related to an excitatory/inhibitory imbalance in ASD. However, our correlational analyses did not demonstrate any significant associations between reduced alpha levels and region-specific differences in the concentrations of either GABA+ or Glx in ASD. However, increased rFEF GABA+ concentrations were related to greater posterior alpha power across all participants and within the TD group specifically. Prior multi-method research has demonstrated that activation of frontal-parietal regions associated with attentional networks (including the rFEF) is related to resting and task-related changes in alpha power (e.g., [50,51,52]). These previous findings suggest that attention-related top-down signals may modulate the power of alpha-band activity. Additionally, inter-individual differences in top-down, goal-oriented control of behavior has been shown to be associated with region-specific GABA concentrations in the FEF [53]. Thus, while no differences were present in FEF GABA+ levels across groups, variable concentrations of inhibitory neurotransmitters within the FEF may contribute to individual differences in resting posterior alpha power within the TD (but not the ASD) group.

One other potential explanation for the absence of any correlations within the ASD may be that the regions examined in the present study do not include the generators of alpha activity that have previously been shown to contribute to differences present in ASD. For example, scalp-recorded alpha power may be associated with GABA-mediated thalamic activity [54, 55]. Prior research by Edgar and colleagues [56] reported significant correlations between thalamic volumes and visual alpha power for TD, but not ASD, children and suggested that the thalamus may contribute to alpha power differences in ASD. Additionally, fMRI evidence indicates that over-connectivity between the thalamus and cortex may be present in ASD [57, 58], and a recent multimodal fMRI-EEG study suggests that children with ASD with the largest reductions in alpha power may also tend to show greatest thalamo-cortical overconnectivity [59]. Thus, while there is limited evidence that GABA and Glx values within the thalamus may not differ in ASD [60,61,62], future work investigating the associations between neurochemical profiles and band-specific power differences may benefit from examination of thalamic contributions to atypical alpha activity in ASD.

Lastly, although we observed associations between alpha levels and Sensitivity and Seeking scores within the TD group, we did not find evidence of a relationship between alpha levels and sensory symptoms within children with ASD. Previous research demonstrating links between alpha and sensory symptoms has focused on frontal alpha asymmetry rather than absolute alpha power [27, 28]. Thus, hemispheric variations in alpha may be more sensitive to differences in sensory processing symptoms rather than absolute levels.

GABA, glutamate, and sensory symptoms in ASD

As previously reported by Edmondson and colleagues [42], only Glx values in the rTPJ differed significantly in our sample of children with ASD, with no differences in GABA+ or Glx values present in any other VOI (see Edmondson et al. [42], for further discussion on the lack of between-group differences and discussion of other metabolites). Although Bernardi and colleagues [62] previously reported equivalent TPJ Glx levels, they did report reduced Glx values in the anterior cingulate cortex. Moreover, our finding of significantly lower Glx is consistent with several other studies that have documented reduced concentrations of Glx in ASD [63,64,65,66].

Contrary to prior research that has demonstrated associations between GABA levels and sensory processing in ASD [33,34,35], no associations were found between GABA+ values in any VOI and sensory processing symptoms across all participants or within TD or ASD groups. Nevertheless, our results are consistent with a recent study that found no association between GABA+ concentrations and behavioral and fMRI measures of visual spatial suppression [67]. Rather, we observed significant correlations between rTPJ Glx levels and all four sensory processing quadrants across both groups, which was primarily the result of associations within the ASD group, for Sensitivity and Avoidance subscales; greater scores on these subscales, indicative of increased hypersensitivity to sensory input, were associated with atypically decreased Glx levels in the ASD group, suggesting that reduced glutamate may contribute to sensory processing impairments in ASD.

Why are atypically decreased Glx values within the rTPJ associated with increased hyper-sensitivity in ASD? The rTPJ is a hub for social-cognitive processes and may coordinate communication about the information gathered from one’s external sensory environment with internal model-based predictions [68]. Further, the TPJ is involved in the processing of auditory, visual, and tactile sensory inputs (e.g., [69]) and is part of a larger brain network involved in the detection of novel multimodal stimuli [70, 71]. Processing of multisensory information may be disrupted in ASD [72], and others have hypothesized that deficits in multisensory integration may lead to a disorganized or chaotic perception of one’s sensory environment and sensory hyper-sensitivity [73]. Thus, neurochemical perturbations, specifically decreased glutamate within the rTPJ, may impact processing of multisensory information resulting in greater reactivity to sensory information.

In addition, a predictive coding account has also been used to explain differences in responding to sensory input in individuals with ASD (e.g., [74,75,76,77]). In particular, Lawson and colleagues [75] hypothesize that context-insensitive sensory drive and failures to optimize precision may result from atypically reduced glutamate in ASD. The mismatch-negativity (MMN) event-related potential (ERP) component, which is elicited by any perceivable change in a stream of repetitive sensory simulation, is thought to reflect early sensory prediction error processing [78]. In TD individuals, higher levels of Glx in the posterior superior temporal gyrus (which was also included in our TPJ VOI) are associated with faster MMN latency, suggesting that increased glutamate levels are related to more efficient prediction error signaling [79]. More recently, Kompus and colleagues [80] also demonstrated that increased levels of temporal Glx were associated with increased inter-regional functional connectivity between auditory cortex and the inferior parietal lobe, but not local connectivity within the temporal lobe, during unpredictable auditory stimulation, demonstrating the importance of Glx in facilitating long-range connectivity. Although the prior findings are mixed, a recent meta-analysis showed that smaller MMN amplitudes are consistently observed in individuals with ASD [81]. Importantly, atypically slower MMF (the magnetic equivalent to the MMN) latency [82] and smaller MMN amplitude [83] have been shown to be related to increased sensory sensitivity scores in ASD. Furthermore, Goris and colleagues [84] demonstrated that MMN amplitude was less modulated by global context in individuals with ASD, supporting predictive coding accounts of ASD. Thus, while it remains to be determined, disrupted glutamatergic signaling within the temporal-parietal cortex may contribute to previous reports of atypical MMN responses, and provide support for the hypoglutamatergic basis for the aberrant precision account of ASD.

Limitations

Children and adolescents with ASD in the present study did provide significantly less usable EEG data; however, the amount of usable EEG was not associated with average alpha power across the two groups, nor did results change when percentage of usable data was included as a covariate. Additionally, because only a subset of participants completed the MRS portion of the study, our MRS analyses were limited to restricted number of participants. As such, correlations with MRS measures should be confirmed with a larger cohort of participants. Furthermore, correlational analyses were not corrected for multiple comparisons, and, thus, should be considered exploratory in nature. Finally, our interpretation of Glx values focuses on glutamate; however, Glx includes contributions from both glutamate and glutamine, and so should be interpreted with caution.

Conclusions

Sensory processing impairments have a significant impact on the quality of life of individuals with ASD and their families. Thus, discovering the neurophysiological underpinnings of ASD symptomatology, including sensory processing differences, has the potential to provide insight into the heterogeneous nature of the ASD phenotype as well as to inform the identification of early diagnostic markers and development of novel intervention approaches. This study presents evidence that resting-state alpha levels are significantly reduced in children and adolescents with ASD compared to their TD peers, supporting a growing body of previous research. However, these differences were not associated inter-individual differences in sensory processing symptoms or region-specific variations in excitatory or inhibitory neurotransmitters. Instead, individuals with ASD exhibited reduced concentrations of Glx in the right temporal-parietal junction, and atypically decreased levels of Glx were associated with greater levels of sensory symptoms. These exploratory findings, which should be confirmed using a larger sample, suggest that a reduction in glutamatergic drive within the temporal-parietal cortex may contribute to hypersensitivity to sensory input in ASD.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed in the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ADOS:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

CSF:

Cerebral-spinal fluid

DAS:

Differential Abilities Scales, Second Edition

EEG:

Electroencepholography

E/I:

Excitatory/inhibitory

ERP:

Event-related potential

fMRI:

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

FEF:

Frontal eye fields

GABA:

Gamma-aminobutyric acid

Glx:

Glutamate + glutamine

ICA:

Independent component analysis

MMN:

Mismatch negativity

MMF:

Mismatch field

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

MRS:

Magnetic resonance spectroscopy

ROI:

Region of interest

SASICA:

Semiautomatic selection of independent components for artifact

SCQ:

Social Communication Questionnaire

SP-2:

Sensory Profile, Second Edition

TD:

Typically developing

TPJ:

Temporal-parietal junction

VIS:

Visual cortex

VOI:

Volume of interest

WASI-II:

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Second Edition

References

  1. Maenner MJ, Shaw KA, Baio J, Washington A, Patrick M, DiRienzo M, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2016. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2020;69(4):1–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. APA. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington D.C.: American Psychological Association; 2013.

  3. Baranek GT, David FJ, Poe MD, Stone WL, Watson LR. Sensory experiences questionnaire: discriminating sensory features in young children with autism, developmental delays, and typical development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):591–601.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Wolff JJ, Dimian AF, Botteron KN, Dager SR, Elison JT, Estes AM, et al. A longitudinal study of parent-reported sensory responsiveness in toddlers at-risk for autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2019;60(3):314–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Baranek GT, Watson LR, Boyd BA, Poe MD, David FJ, McGuire L. Hyporesponsiveness to social and nonsocial sensory stimuli in children with autism, children with developmental delays, and typically developing children. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(2):307–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Harris KD, Thiele A. Cortical state and attention. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(9):509–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Herrmann CS, Struber D, Helfrich RF, Engel AK. EEG oscillations: from correlation to causality. Int J Psychophysiol. 2016;103:12–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Foxe JJ, Snyder AC. The role of alpha-band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. Front Psychol. 2011;2:154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Klimesch W. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(12):606–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Hanslmayr S, Aslan A, Staudigl T, Klimesch W, Herrmann CS, Bauml KH. Prestimulus oscillations predict visual perception performance between and within subjects. Neuroimage. 2007;37(4):1465–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Zhang Y, Ding M. Detection of a weak somatosensory stimulus: role of the prestimulus mu rhythm and its top-down modulation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22(2):307–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Mathewson KE, Gratton G, Fabiani M, Beck DM, Ro T. To see or not to see: prestimulus alpha phase predicts visual awareness. J Neurosci. 2009;29(9):2725–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Jensen O, Mazaheri A. Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;4:186.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Mathewson KE, Lleras A, Beck DM, Fabiani M, Ro T, Gratton G. Pulsed out of awareness: EEG alpha oscillations represent a pulsed-inhibition of ongoing cortical processing. Front Psychol. 2011;2:99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Wang J, Barstein J, Ethridge LE, Mosconi MW, Takarae Y, Sweeney JA. Resting state EEG abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders. J Neurodev Disord. 2013;5(1):24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Levin AR, Varcin KJ, O'Leary HM, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. EEG power at 3 months in infants at high familial risk for autism. J Neurodev Disord. 2017;9(1):34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Tierney AL, Gabard-Durnam L, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Developmental trajectories of resting EEG power: an endophenotype of autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One. 2012;7(6):e39127.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Keehn B, Westerfield M, Müller R-A, Townsend J. Autism, attention, and alpha oscillations: an electrophysiological study of attentional capture. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2017.

  19. DiStefano C, Dickinson A, Baker E, Jeste SS. EEG data collection in children with ASD: the role of state in data quality and spectral power. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2019;57:132–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Shephard E, Tye C, Ashwood KL, Azadi B, Asherson P, Bolton PF, et al. Resting-state neurophysiological activity patterns in young people with ASD, ADHD, and ASD + ADHD. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(1):110–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Dawson G, Klinger LG, Panagiotides H, Lewy A, Castelloe P. Subgroups of autistic children based on social behavior display distinct patterns of brain activity. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1995;23(5):569–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Murias M, Webb SJ, Greenson J, Dawson G. Resting state cortical connectivity reflected in EEG coherence in individuals with autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(3):270–3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Cornew L, Roberts TP, Blaskey L, Edgar JC. Resting-state oscillatory activity in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(9):1884–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Mathewson KJ, Jetha MK, Drmic IE, Bryson SE, Goldberg JO, Schmidt LA. Regional EEG alpha power, coherence, and behavioral symptomatology in autism spectrum disorder. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012;123(9):1798–809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Sutton SK, Burnette CP, Mundy PC, Meyer J, Vaughan A, Sanders C, et al. Resting cortical brain activity and social behavior in higher functioning children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(2):211–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Coben R, Clarke AR, Hudspeth W, Barry RJ. EEG power and coherence in autistic spectrum disorder. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119(5):1002–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Simon DM, Damiano CR, Woynaroski TG, Ibanez LV, Murias M, Stone WL, et al. Neural correlates of sensory hyporesponsiveness in toddlers at high risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47(9):2710–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Damiano-Goodwin CR, Woynaroski TG, Simon DM, Ibanez LV, Murias M, Kirby A, et al. Developmental sequelae and neurophysiologic substrates of sensory seeking in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;29:41–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Simon DM, Wallace MT. Dysfunction of sensory oscillations in autism spectrum disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;68:848–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Hussman JP. Suppressed GABAergic inhibition as a common factor in suspected etiologies of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2001;31(2):247–8.

  31. Rubenstein JL, Merzenich MM. Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2003;2(5):255–67.

  32. Ajram LA, Pereira AC, Durieux AMS, Velthius HE, Petrinovic MM, McAlonan GM. The contribution of [1H] magnetic resonance spectroscopy to the study of excitation-inhibition in autism. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2019;89:236–44.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Puts NAJ, Wodka EL, Harris AD, Crocetti D, Tommerdahl M, Mostofsky SH, et al. Reduced GABA and altered somatosensory function in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2017;10(4):608–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Sapey-Triomphe LA, Lamberton F, Sonie S, Mattout J, Schmitz C. Tactile hypersensitivity and GABA concentration in the sensorimotor cortex of adults with autism. Autism Res. 2019;12(4):562–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Umesawa Y, Atsumi T, Chakrabarty M, Fukatsu R, Ide M. GABA concentration in the left ventral premotor cortex associates with sensory hyper-responsiveness in autism spectrum disorders without intellectual disability. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:482.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Wechsler D. Wechsler’s abbreviated scale of intelligence - second edition (WASI-II). NCS Pearson: San Antonio, TX; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Elliott C. Differential abilities scales - section edition. Assessment H, editor. San Antonio, TX2007.

  38. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnostic Obervation schedule. Second ed. Western Psychological Services: Torrance, CA; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Rutter M, Bailey A, Lord C. Social communication questionnaire (SCQ). Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Delorme A, Mullen T, Kothe C, Akalin Acar Z, Bigdely-Shamlo N, Vankov A, et al. EEGLAB, SIFT, NFT, BCILAB, and ERICA: new tools for advanced EEG processing. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:130714.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Chaumon M, Bishop DV, Busch NA. A practical guide to the selection of independent components of the electroencephalogram for artifact correction. J Neurosci Methods. 2015;250:47–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Edmondson DA, Xia P, McNally Keehn R, Dydak U, Keehn B. A magnetic resonance spectroscopy study of superior visual search abilities in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2020;13(4):550–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Andreychenko A, Boer VO. Arteaga de Castro CS, Luijten PR, Klomp DW. Efficient spectral editing at 7 T: GABA detection with MEGA-sLASER. Magn Reson Med. 2012;68(4):1018–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Marjanska M, Lehericy S, Valabregue R, Popa T, Worbe Y, Russo M, et al. Brain dynamic neurochemical changes in dystonic patients: a magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Mov Disord. 2013;28(2):201–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Provencher SW. Estimation of metabolite concentrations from localized in vivo proton NMR spectra. Magn Reson Med. 1993;30(6):672–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Kaiser LG, Young K, Matson GB. Elimination of spatial interference in PRESS-localized editing spectroscopy. Magn Reson Med. 2007;58(4):813–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Harris AD, Puts NA, Edden RA. Tissue correction for GABA-edited MRS: considerations of voxel composition, tissue segmentation, and tissue relaxations. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2015;42(5):1431–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Dunn W. Sensory profile - 2. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Publishing; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Corcoran AW, Alday PM, Schlesewsky M, Bornkessel-Schlesewsky I. Toward a reliable, automated method of individual alpha frequency (IAF) quantification. Psychophysiology. 2018;55(7):e13064.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Laufs H, Krakow K, Sterzer P, Eger E, Beyerle A, Salek-Haddadi A, et al. Electroencephalographic signatures of attentional and cognitive default modes in spontaneous brain activity fluctuations at rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(19):11053–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Sadaghiani S, Scheeringa R, Lehongre K, Morillon B, Giraud AL, Kleinschmidt A. Intrinsic connectivity networks, alpha oscillations, and tonic alertness: a simultaneous electroencephalography/functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2010;30(30):10243–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Mathewson KE, Beck DM, Ro T, Maclin EL, Low KA, Fabiani M, et al. Dynamics of alpha control: preparatory suppression of posterior alpha oscillations by frontal modulators revealed with combined EEG and event-related optical signal. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014;26(10):2400–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Sumner P, Edden RA, Bompas A, Evans CJ, Singh KD. More GABA, less distraction: a neurochemical predictor of motor decision speed. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(7):825–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Schreckenberger M, Lange-Asschenfeldt C, Lochmann M, Mann K, Siessmeier T, Buchholz HG, et al. The thalamus as the generator and modulator of EEG alpha rhythm: a combined PET/EEG study with lorazepam challenge in humans. Neuroimage. 2004;22(2):637–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Lorincz ML, Kekesi KA, Juhasz G, Crunelli V, Hughes SW. Temporal framing of thalamic relay-mode firing by phasic inhibition during the alpha rhythm. Neuron. 2009;63(5):683–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Edgar JC, Heiken K, Chen YH, Herrington JD, Chow V, Liu S, et al. Resting-state alpha in autism spectrum disorder and alpha associations with thalamic volume. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(3):795–804.

  57. Cerliani L, Mennes M, Thomas RM, Di Martino A, Thioux M, Keysers C. Increased functional connectivity between subcortical and cortical resting-state networks in autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(8):767–77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Woodward ND, Giraldo-Chica M, Rogers B, Cascio CJ. Thalamocortical dysconnectivity in autism spectrum disorder: an analysis of the autism brain imaging data exchange. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2017;2(1):76–84.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Mash LE, Keehn B, Linke AC, Liu TT, Helm JL, Haist F, et al. Atypical relationships between spontaneous EEG and fMRI activity in autism. Brain Connect. 2020;10(1):18–28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Fung LK, Flores RE, Gu M, Sun KL, James D, Schuck RK, et al. Thalamic and prefrontal GABA concentrations but not GABAA receptor densities are altered in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2020.

  61. Hardan AY, Minshew NJ, Melhem NM, Srihari S, Jo B, Bansal R, et al. An MRI and proton spectroscopy study of the thalamus in children with autism. Psychiatry Res. 2008;163(2):97–105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Bernardi S, Anagnostou E, Shen J, Kolevzon A, Buxbaum JD, Hollander E, et al. In vivo 1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy study of the attentional networks in autism. Brain Res. 2011;1380:198–205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Tebartz van Elst L, Maier S, Fangmeier T, Endres D, Mueller GT, Nickel K, et al. Disturbed cingulate glutamate metabolism in adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: evidence in support of the excitatory/inhibitory imbalance hypothesis. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(12):1314–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. DeVito TJ, Drost DJ, Neufeld RW, Rajakumar N, Pavlosky W, Williamson P, et al. Evidence for cortical dysfunction in autism: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(4):465–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Horder J, Lavender T, Mendez MA, O'Gorman R, Daly E, Craig MC, et al. Reduced subcortical glutamate/glutamine in adults with autism spectrum disorders: a [(1)H]MRS study. Transl Psychiatry. 2014;4:e364.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Horder J, Petrinovic MM, Mendez MA, Bruns A, Takumi T, Spooren W, et al. Glutamate and GABA in autism spectrum disorder-a translational magnetic resonance spectroscopy study in man and rodent models. Transl Psychiatry. 2018;8(1):106.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. Schallmo MP, Kolodny T, Kale AM, Millin R, Flevaris AV, Edden RAE, et al. Weaker neural suppression in autism. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):2675.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Patel GH, Sestieri C, Corbetta M. The evolution of the temporoparietal junction and posterior superior temporal sulcus. Cortex. 2019;118:38–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Matsuhashi M, Ikeda A, Ohara S, Matsumoto R, Yamamoto J, Takayama M, et al. Multisensory convergence at human temporo-parietal junction - epicortical recording of evoked responses. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115(5):1145–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Downar J, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD. A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(3):277–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Downar J, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD. A cortical network sensitive to stimulus salience in a neutral behavioral context across multiple sensory modalities. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87(1):615–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Baum SH, Stevenson RA, Wallace MT. Behavioral, perceptual, and neural alterations in sensory and multisensory function in autism spectrum disorder. Prog Neurobiol. 2015;134:140–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Brandwein AB, Foxe JJ, Butler JS, Frey HP, Bates JC, Shulman LH, et al. Neurophysiological indices of atypical auditory processing and multisensory integration are associated with symptom severity in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(1):230–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Van de Cruys S, Evers K, Van der Hallen R, Van Eylen L, Boets B. de-wit L, et al. precise minds in uncertain worlds: predictive coding in autism. Psychol Rev. 2014;121(4):649–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Lawson RP, Rees G, Friston KJ. An aberrant precision account of autism. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:302.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Courchesne E, Allen G. Prediction and preparation, fundamental functions of the cerebellum. Learn Mem. 1997;4(1):1–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Sinha P, Kjelgaard MM, Gandhi TK, Tsourides K, Cardinaux AL, Pantazis D, et al. Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(42):15220–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Friston K. A theory of cortical responses. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2005;360(1456):815–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Kompus K, Westerhausen R, Craven AR, Kreegipuu K, Poldver N, Passow S, et al. Resting-state glutamatergic neurotransmission is related to the peak latency of the auditory mismatch negativity (MMN) for duration deviants: an (1)H-MRS-EEG study. Psychophysiology. 2015;52(9):1131–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Kompus K, Volehaugen V, Craven A, Specht K. Glutamatergic modulation of auditory cortex connectivity with attentional brain networks in unpredictable perceptual environment. bioRxiv. 2019:2019.12.20.884049.

  81. Schwartz S, Shinn-Cunningham B, Tager-Flusberg H. Meta-analysis and systematic review of the literature characterizing auditory mismatch negativity in individuals with autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;87:106–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Matsuzaki J, Kagitani-Shimono K, Sugata H, Hanaie R, Nagatani F, Yamamoto T, et al. Delayed mismatch field latencies in autism spectrum disorder with abnormal auditory sensitivity: a magnetoencephalographic study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11:446.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Ludlow A, Mohr B, Whitmore A, Garagnani M, Pulvermuller F, Gutierrez R. Auditory processing and sensory behaviours in children with autism spectrum disorders as revealed by mismatch negativity. Brain Cogn. 2014;86:55–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Goris J, Braem S, Nijhof AD, Rigoni D, Deschrijver E, Van de Cruys S, et al. Sensory prediction errors are less modulated by global context in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018;3(8):667–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Special thanks to the children and families who generously participated and Sophia Bergmann for assistance with data collection. Furthermore we acknowledge the receipt of the sLASER and MEGA-sLASER sequences by the Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, University of Minnesota, developed by Edward Auerbach and Malgorzata Marjanska.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIH/NIMH) R21 MH114095 (BK) and the Purdue Institute for Integrative Neuroscience (#209257; BK, UD). MRI data acquisition was supported in part by NIH S10 OD012336 (UD).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SP, UD, and BK conceived of the study. GK, RMK, and BK collected the EEG and phenotypic data. DAE, UD, and BK developed the MRS protocol and collected the MRS data. SP completed the data analysis pipeline for the EEG and DAE completed the data analysis pipeline for the MRS data. SP and BK analyzed the data and drafted the initial version of the manuscript. All authors reviewed and revised the manuscript and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brandon Keehn.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All study procedures were approved by the Purdue University Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained from all caregivers, and written assent was obtained from all children and adolescents.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Supplementary Tables

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pierce, S., Kadlaskar, G., Edmondson, D.A. et al. Associations between sensory processing and electrophysiological and neurochemical measures in children with ASD: an EEG-MRS study. J Neurodevelop Disord 13, 5 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-020-09351-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-020-09351-0

Keywords