Skip to main content

Social cognition in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and idiopathic developmental neuropsychiatric disorders

Abstract

Background

22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS) is a common recurrent neurogenetic condition associated with elevated risk for developmental neuropsychiatric disorders and intellectual disability. Children and adults with 22q11DS often exhibit marked social impairment as well as neurocognitive deficits, and have elevated rates of both autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and psychosis. However, the relationship between the basic processes of social cognition and cognitive ability has not been well studied in 22q11DS. Here, we examined differences in social cognition in 22q11DS, relative to multiple groups of idiopathic neuropsychiatric disorders, and typically developing healthy controls (HC). Additionally, we examined differences in intellectual functioning and its relationship to social cognitive abilities. Finally, we examined the relationship between social cognitive abilities and real-world social behavior.

Methods

We examined social cognition and intellectual functioning in 273 participants (mean age = 17.74 ± 5.18% female = 44.3%): 50 with 22q11DS, 49 youth with first episode psychosis (FEP), 48 at clinical high-risk (CHR) for psychosis, 24 participants with ASD, and 102 HC. Social cognition was assessed using The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT), while reciprocal social behavior was assessed via parent/caregiver ratings on the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS). Participants were also administered the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, 2nd edition (WASI-II) to assess intellectual functioning.

Results

The 22q11DS group exhibited significantly lower social cognitive abilities compared to CHR, FEP, and HC groups after controlling for intellectual functioning, but not in comparison to the ASD group. Significant positive correlations were found between social cognition, as measured by the TASIT and IQ across groups. In contrast, no significant relationships were found between TASIT and real-world social behavior (SRS) for any group.

Conclusions

Our findings indicate social cognitive deficits are more prominent in 22q11DS than idiopathic neuropsychiatric conditions across the age range, even after adjusting for global intellectual function. These results contribute to our understanding of the intellectual and social vulnerabilities of 22q11DS in comparison to idiopathic neuropsychiatric disorders. Our findings of robust associations between intellectual ability and social cognition emphasizes the importance of accounting for neurocognitive deficits in social skills interventions and tailoring these existing treatment models for 22q11DS and other populations with intellectual impairment.

Background

22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS; also known as DiGeorge or Velocardiofacial Syndrome) is caused by a microdeletion of ~ 46 protein-coding genes on the long arm of chromosome 22 [1] and is the most common recurrent contiguous gene deletion syndrome, estimated to occur in about 1 in 4000 live births [2]. It typically results from a de novo deletion, although 10% of cases are familial [3, 4]. 22q11DS is associated with a range of developmental anomalies [5]. The most frequently associated medical comorbidities include congenital heart defects and facial dysmorphology, such as palatal defects, immunodeficiency, velopharyngeal insufficiency, and hypocalcemia [2, 6].

Individuals with 22q11DS often present with developmental delays, as well as broad cognitive impairment [6]. In most individuals with 22q11DS, Full-Scale IQ falls within the intellectually disabled to low average range (M = 72.41, SD = + 13.72) [7]. A range of associated cognitive deficits have also been noted in individuals with 22q11DS, particularly difficulties in working memory, attention, executive functioning, visuospatial processing, learning and memory, arithmetic, and sensorimotor abilities [8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15]. These cognitive impairments can often cascade into significant learning difficulties at school, as well as challenges with adaptive life skills including socio-emotional functioning.

In recent years, several studies have characterized deficits in social behavior and social cognition in individuals with 22q11DS. Specifically, these studies have indicated that youth with 22q11DS have difficulties initiating and maintaining social relationships [5, 16]. In the laboratory, individuals with 22q11DS show impairments in identifying facial displays of emotion and a tendency to demonstrate eye-gaze preferences for the mouth over the eyes [9, 17, 18]. Additionally, other studies found that individuals with 22q11DS were significantly less accurate than healthy controls in identifying or matching emotions [1, 9, 19], particularly when identifying fear, anger, and disgust [9, 17]. Furthermore, 22q11DS individuals have demonstrated poorer performance on theory of mind (ToM) tasks wherein they were required to infer the beliefs, attitudes, and intentions of other people [19,20,21]. However, differences in non-social neurocognitive abilities, such as executive functioning, could in part account for differences in social cognitive performance [5, 15]. Prior studies have also found that impairments in both social cognition and social skills are more pronounced in children and adolescents with 22q11DS than in children with general developmental delays or idiopathic intellectual disability, as well as compared to other genetic conditions such as Williams syndrome [22,23,24].

In addition, 22q11DS is associated with increased incidence of developmental neuropsychiatric disorders across the lifespan [25]. Based on existing literature, rates of clinical diagnoses within the 22q11DS population are estimated to be 25–50% for autism spectrum disorder [26,27,28], 30–40% for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder [29], 20–35% for psychotic disorders, 30–40% for anxiety disorders, and 20–30% for mood disorders [30]. Conversely, about 0.3% of schizophrenia cases in the general population are estimated to carry a 22q11.2 deletion [31].

All of the aforementioned co-morbid neuropsychiatric conditions are also characterized by social cognitive deficits [32]. However, little is known about how these social impairments and broader neurocognitive profiles are the same or different for 22q11DS compared to primary idiopathic developmental neuropsychiatric disorders. One prior study found poorer adaptive social skills in individuals with 22q11DS and psychotic symptoms, compared to 22q11DS youth without psychosis [33]. To the best of our knowledge, however, no published studies to date have investigated differences in social cognition profiles in individuals with primary idiopathic psychotic spectrum conditions or ASD [34, 35] compared to a highly penetrant genetic condition such as 22q11DS. Thus, examining social cognition and its relationship to global intellectual function and social behavior in 22q11DS relative to idiopathic neuropsychiatric disorders can facilitate development of more targeted treatment options, and as a result, improve overall quality of life for this group. Towards this aim, in the current study, we sought to examine patterns of social cognition deficits in a large cohort of individuals with 22q11DS, relative to youth with a recent onset of subclinical psychotic-like symptoms (i.e., a clinical high-risk (CHR) group), youth with first episode psychosis, youth with ASD, and typically developing healthy controls. Specifically, we administered an audiovisual task of social perception that examines one’s ability to interpret another person’s thoughts, intentions, and feelings [36, 37]. A further goal was to examine differences in intellectual functioning among these groups, and its relationship with social cognition performance. Finally, we assessed the relationship of social cognitive task performance to parent/caregiver report of reciprocal social behavior across groups. We hypothesized that (1) individuals with 22q11DS would have significantly poorer social cognition task performance compared to all other clinical groups, even after controlling for intellectual functioning; (2) social cognition would be associated with intellectual abilities across all groups; and (3) social cognitive task performance across groups would be associated with parent/caregiver report of ‘real world’ reciprocal social behavior.

Methods

Participants

Our sample consisted of participants aged 12 to 35, enrolled in multiple research studies at the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) that used parallel procedures to assess neurocognition and psychopathology. In total, the current analysis includes 273 participants: 50 patients with 22q11.2 deletions (22 males, 28 females), 48 individuals at clinical high risk (CHR) for psychosis (35 males, 13 females), 49 individuals with first-episode psychosis (FEP; 27 males, 22 females), 24 individuals with ASD (20 males, 4 females), and 102 healthy controls (HC; 48 males, 54 females; see Table 1). All participants were recruited via voluntary response sampling techniques through posting of recruitment flyers at various UCLA psychiatric and genetic clinics, as well as our outreach efforts at various public events in the Southern California region. Typically, participants are referred to our research programs (described below) by providers or contact our study staff directly due to our outreach and recruitment efforts including the HC group. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants, and additionally by parents/caregivers for participants under the age of 18, using procedures approved by the Institutional Review Board at UCLA.

Table 1 Demographic data for participant groups

Patients with 22q11DS were recruited from an ongoing UCLA study, with all patients having a molecularly confirmed diagnosis of 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. CHR individuals were recruited from the UCLA Center for the Assessment and Prevention of Prodromal States (CAPPS), a clinical research center that specializes in identifying and treating prodromal adolescents and young adults at high risk for developing psychotic disorders [38,39,40]. CHR participants met criteria for one of three psychosis risk syndromes, as assessed by the Structured Interview for Prodromal States (SIPS [41];, based on (1) attenuated (sub-threshold) psychotic symptoms; (2) transient, recent-onset psychotic symptoms; or (3) a substantial drop in social/role functioning in conjunction with Schizotypal Personality Disorder diagnosis or presence of psychotic disorder in first-degree relative. FEP individuals were recruited from the UCLA Adolescent Brain-Behavior Research Center (ABBRC). Inclusion criteria for FEP participants involved a diagnosis of psychotic disorder (including schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder, schizoaffective disorder, and psychotic disorder not otherwise specified), confirmed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I diagnoses (SCID [42];) by trained study personnel. Individuals with ASD were recruited from an ongoing research study at UCLA’s Center for Autism Research and Treatment (CART). Participants were assessed for ASD symptomatology using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2 [43];) by trained clinicians. HC were recruited from each of these ongoing studies, with similar inclusion and exclusion criteria: specifically, HC subjects could not have a personal or close family history of any major neurodevelopmental or psychiatric disorder or intellectual disability (IQ < 70). All CHR, FEP, and ASD, and HC participants were verbally screened for known genetic or medical conditions potentially associated with secondary psychosis or ASD [34, 35], and excluded based on known conditions. However, we did not require all our CHR, FEP, ASD, and HC participants to undergo genetic testing to confirm the absence of underlying genetic conditions. Additional exclusion criteria for all groups included significant neurological or medical conditions (not attributable to 22q11.2 deletion), and/or substance use disorder in the past 6 months.

Cognitive measures

The following assessments were administered to participants within each group across all studies. General intellectual functioning was estimated using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, 2nd edition (WASI-II [44];). This included Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ), Verbal IQ (VIQ) estimated using the Vocabulary subtest, and Non-verbal IQ (NVIQ) estimated using the Matrix Reasoning subtest. To assess social cognition, all participants were administered The Awareness of Social Inference Test-Part 3 (TASIT [36, 37];). The TASIT consisted of 16 video-taped vignettes (each lasting between 15 to 60 s) of everyday social situations, assessing one’s ability to draw inferences about the thoughts, intentions, beliefs, and feelings of others engaged in conversational exchanges involving white lies or sarcasm. After viewing each vignette, participants were asked to answer four types of yes or no questions addressing different aspects of communicative intentions of the characters. The first question asked what one character in the scene was doing (Do), the second question asked what the character was trying to say to the other person (Say), the third question asked what the main character was thinking (Think), and the fourth question asked what the main character was feeling (Feel). A TASIT total score was calculated for each participant by adding their responses to each of the 4 questions per scene across all 16 vignettes, ranging from 0 to 64. Additionally, parents/caregivers of 22q11DS, CHR, ASD, and HC participants were administered the Social Responsiveness Scale-2nd Edition (SRS [45];)—a parent/caregiver rating scale to quantify (dimensionally measured) social impairments within the context of the ASD phenotype [46]. The SRS-2 includes the following subscales: Social Awareness, Social Cognition, Social Communication, Social Motivation, and Restricted Interests and Repetitive Behavior. Parents/caregivers of the FEP group were administered the original version of the SRS [47], which included the following subscales: Receptive, Cognitive, Expressive, Motivational Aspects of Social Behavior, and Autistic Preoccupations. The original SRS administered to parents/caregivers of the FEP group was rescored using the updated norms for the revised version (SRS-2), both of which have the same items, so as to be consistent across all groups. The SRS was originally developed to quantify (dimensionally measured) social impairments within the context of the ASD phenotype, but has now been extended to many other clinical populations, including 22q11DS [20, 48], CHR [21, 49, 50], and FEP [21, 49]. It has also been shown to index traits that are continuously distributed in the general population [51]. Both versions of the SRS are 65-item questionnaires utilizing a 4-point Likert scale (0 = not true, 1 = sometimes true, 2 = often true, 3 = almost always true), with a total T-score computed across all domains. Both versions of the SRS are identical for ages 4 through 18, although the subscales are labeled differently. There is also considerable overlap between the original SRS and the revised Adult Form for ages 19 and over, despite some items that differ on the second edition [52]. All the participants in this study, including adult participants, lived with the parents/caregivers who completed SRS reports on them.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) software v. 25 (Armonk, NY, USA). First, we compared demographic variables between groups using independent sample t tests for continuous variables such as age and socio-economic status (SES) indexed as mean parental education years, and chi-square tests for categorical variables such as sex and race/ethnicity. Participants significantly differed on age, sex distribution, mean parental education, and race (Table 1); i.e., CHR and HC participants were significantly older, and CHR youth had significantly lower mean parental education than the other groups. There were also proportionally fewer female participants in the ASD group relative to the other groups. Additionally, the 22q11DS group had significantly more participants who identified as white/non-Hispanic, while the FEP group had significantly more participants who identified as being of Hispanic origin compared to all other groups. The HC group had more participants that identified with other racial/ethnicity categories. Hence, these variables were included as covariates in all subsequent group analyses. Second, one-way analysis of covariances (ANCOVAs), in which we adjusted for age, sex, SES, and race/ethnicity, were performed to examine between-group differences in total TASIT score. We followed up on significant overall differences by examining between-group differences in subdomain TASIT scores (Do, Say, Think, and Feel). Following this, exploratory post-hoc analyses were conducted to elucidate directionality of group differences for each significant ANCOVA. Third, we performed ANCOVAs on the WASI-II scores to examine differences in FSIQ, VIQ, and NVIQ between groups. We then performed an ANCOVA to examine group differences in TASIT total performance after additionally controlling for FSIQ scores. To investigate the relationship between IQ and TASIT performance, we conducted Pearson’s correlations between subscores on these measures. Next, as our 22q11DS sample included individuals with comorbid diagnoses of ASD and psychosis, we conducted a follow-up analysis of group differences in TASIT and IQ scores in diagnostic subgroups within our 22q11DS sample: 22q11DS with a comorbid ASD diagnosis (N = 17), 22q11DS with a comorbid psychosis diagnosis (N = 2), 22q11DS with both psychosis and ASD (N = 5), and 22q11DS with no psychosis or ASD diagnosis (N = 26). We also performed additional follow-up analysis of group differences in TASIT and IQ scores in idiopathic ASD compared to 22q11DS with a comorbid ASD only diagnosis and 22q11DS with no comorbid ASD or psychosis diagnosis (See Supplementary Material). Finally, we examined between-group differences in parent-report of real-world social behavior, assessed via the SRS total and subscale scores, using one-way ANCOVA, and examined the relationship between TASIT performance and SRS total score using Spearman’s correlations, since the latter scores were not normally distributed. All ANCOVAs and correlational analyses described above were adjusted for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction for total number of comparisons. All significance values reported below are Bonferroni adjusted p values.

Results

Social cognition results

Results from the first one-way ANCOVA revealed significant group differences for total TASIT score [F (4, 268) = 30.912, p < .01]. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that all groups obtained significantly higher total TASIT scores compared to the 22q11DS group [22q11DS<CHR: (t(96) = − 7.991, p < .01); 22q11DS<FEP: (t(97) = − 3.346, p < .01); 22q11DS<ASD: (t(72) = − 4.497, p < .01); 22q11DS<HC: (t(150) = − 12.528, p < .01)]. Additionally, FEP and ASD groups obtained significantly lower total TASIT scores than the HC group (Fig. 1a) [HC>FEP: (t(149) = 5.459, p < .01); HC>ASD: (t(124) = 4.306, p = .01)]. Follow-up one-way ANCOVAs revealed that the 22q11DS group obtained significantly lower scores across all four subdomains of the TASIT compared to all other groups [Fig. 1b–e]. Additionally, the FEP group obtained significantly lower Do and Say scores than HCs (Fig. 1b, c). The 22q11DS group obtained significantly lower Think scores than CHR, ASD, and HC groups, but did not differ from the FEP group, who in turn performed more poorly than the HC group (Fig. 1d). Additionally, the FEP group obtained significantly lower Feel scores than the HCs (Fig. 1e). See Table 2 for means and standard deviations on TASIT total and subscores for each group.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Boxplots showing group differences in TASIT scores. Lower and upper box boundaries indicate 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively, black line inside box represents median, whiskers denote minimum to maximum range, and significant pairwise differences between groups are indicated by asterisks (**=p < .01). a Group differences for total TASIT score (22q11DS<FEP, ASD, and HC; FEP<HC; and ASD<HC). b Group differences in TASIT score for subdomain Do (22q11DS<CHR, FEP, ASD, and HC; FEP<HC); c Group differences in TASIT score for subdomain Say (22q11DS<CHR, FEP, ASD, and HC; FEP<HC). d Group differences in TASIT score for subdomain Think (22q11DS<CHR, ASD, and HC; FEP<HC). e Group differences in TASIT score for subdomain Feel (22q11DS < CHR, FEP, ASD, and HC; FEP<HC)

Table 2 Group means and standard deviations for TASIT (raw scores)

Intellectual functioning (IQ) results

Next, one-way ANCOVAs of WASI-II FSIQ scores revealed a significant overall main effect, indicating significant group differences in FSIQ [Fig. 2; F (4, 265) = 52.106, p < .01]. Post-hoc tests revealed that 22q11DS individuals obtained significantly lower FSIQ scores compared to all other groups [Fig. 2a], and the FEP group obtained significantly lower FSIQ scores than both CHR, ASD, and HC groups. Significant group differences were also observed for VIQ [F (4, 265) = 46.106, p < .01], with 22q11DS individuals obtaining significantly lower VIQ scores compared to all other groups (Fig. 2b). Additionally, the FEP group obtained significantly lower VIQ scores than both CHR and HC groups (p < .01 for both comparisons), but did not differ from the ASD group (p = .50, n.s.). Similarly, 22q11DS individuals obtained significantly lower NVIQ scores than all other groups [F (4, 233) = 45.635, p < .01; Fig. 2c]. Additionally, youth with FEP obtained significantly lower NVIQ scores than those with ASD (Fig. 2c), but did not significantly differ from HC (p = .12, n.s.). See Table 3 for means and standard deviations of WASI-II IQ scores for each group.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Boxplots showing group differences in FSIQ, VIQ, and NVIQ scores. Lower and upper box boundaries indicate 25th and 75th percentiles respectively, black line inside box represents median, whiskers denote minimum to maximum range, and significant pairwise differences between groups are indicated by asterisks (**=p < .01). a Group differences in FSIQ (22q11DS<CHR, FEP, ASD, and HC; FEP<CHR, ASD, and HC). b Group differences in VIQ score (22q11DS<CHR, FEP, ASD, and HC; FEP<CHR and HC). c Group differences in NVIQ score (22q11DS<CHR, FEP, ASD, and HC; FEP<ASD)

Table 3 Group means and standard deviations for WASI-II IQ scores

Social cognition performance controlling for IQ

A follow-up univariate ANCOVA revealed that significant group differences in TASIT performance remained after controlling for IQ [Fig. 3; F (4, 264) = 7.471, p < .01]. Specifically, after controlling for FSIQ, 22q11DS participants still had significantly poorer performance on the TASIT (total score) relative to FEP and HC, but did not differ from CHR (p = .66, n.s.) and ASD (p = .89, n.s.; Fig. 3a). Additionally, controlling for FSIQ, FEP participants still had significantly lower TASIT total scores than CHR, and ASD participants had significantly lower TASIT total scores than CHR and HC (Fig. 3a). Next, we wanted to determine whether controlling for VIQ or NVIQ specifically impacted TASIT performance across groups. After controlling for VIQ, significant group differences in TASIT performance remained [F (4, 264) = 6.119, p < .01], with 22q11DS individuals obtaining significantly lower TASIT total scores than CHR and HC groups, but did not differ from FEP (p = .48, n.s) and ASD group (p = .61, n.s.; Fig. 3b). Additionally, significant between-group differences in TASIT performance was also observed after controlling for NVIQ [F (4, 232) = 10.099, p < .01], with 22q11DS individuals obtaining significantly lower TASIT total scores than CHR, FEP, and HC groups, but not compared to the ASD group (p = .72, n.s.). The ASD group also obtained significantly lower TASIT total scores than both CHR and HC groups, controlling for NVIQ (Fig. 3c).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Boxplots showing group differences in TASIT total score after covarying for FSIQ. Lower and upper box boundaries indicate 25th and 75th percentiles respectively, black line inside box represents median, whiskers denote minimum to maximum range, and significant pairwise differences between groups are indicated by asterisks (**=p < .01). a Group differences in TASIT total score after covarying for WASI-II FSIQ score (22q11DS<FEP and HC; FEP<CHR; ASD<CHR and HC). b Group differences in TASIT total score after covarying for WASI-II VIQ (22q11DS<CHR and HC). c Group differences in TASIT total score after covarying for WASI-II NVIQ (22q11DS<CHR, FEP, and HC; ASD<CHR and HC)

Relationship between social cognition performance and IQ

Pearson’s correlation results indicated positive relationships between TASIT total score and FSIQ as well as VIQ for all groups, such that higher intellectual functioning was associated with better TASIT performance in each group (Fig. 4a, b). Lastly, we found significant positive correlations between TASIT and NVIQ for all groups, except ASD (Fig. 4c).

Fig. 4
figure 4

a Scatterplot showing correlation between TASIT and FSIQ for 22q11DS, CHR, FEP, ASD, and HC. IQ is positively and significantly correlated with TASIT total score for all groups. b Scatterplot showing correlation between TASIT total score and VIQ across groups. VIQ score is positively and significantly correlated with TASIT total score for all groups. c Scatterplot showing correlation between TASIT total score and NVIQ across groups. NVIQ score is positively and significantly correlated with TASIT total score for 22q11DS, CHR, FEP, and HC groups (but not ASD)

Reciprocal social behavior

Results from the one-way ANCOVA revealed significant group differences in SRS total score across groups [Fig. 5a; F (4, 140) = 9,861 p < .01]; HC participants obtained significantly lower SRS total scores compared to all other groups, indicating more normative social behavior (p < .01, for all comparisons), except CHR (p = .07, n.s.). While clinical groups (22q11DS, CHR, FEP, and ASD) obtained clinically elevated mean SRS total scores, there were no significant differences in SRS score between clinical groups (Supplementary Table 1). Lastly, we examined the relationship between TASIT performance and real-world social behavior, as assessed via the SRS, for each group. However, no correlations between these measures survived correction for multiple comparisons (Fig. 5b). We also ran an exploratory analysis examining the association between SRS subscale scores and TASIT total score for each group, and did not find any associations between subscale scores and TASIT performance that survived correction for multiple comparisons (Supplementary Table 2).

Fig. 5
figure 5

a Boxplots showing group differences in SRS total score. Lower and upper box boundaries indicate 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively, black line inside box represents median, whiskers denote minimum to maximum range, and significant pairwise differences between groups are indicated by asterisks (**=p < .01) (HC<22q11DS, FEP, and ASD). b Scatterplot showing correlations between TASIT total score and SRS by group: no significant correlations found

22q11DS subgroup analyses

Within the 22q11DS sample, we found significant differences in VIQ [F (3, 45) = 4.743, p = .01], and relatedly FSIQ [F (3, 45) = 3.397, p = .03], as a function of psychiatric diagnosis, but no differences in NVIQ (p = .29, n.s.). Specifically, 22q11DS patients with both psychosis and ASD obtained significantly lower VIQ (and FSIQ) than the 22q11DS group with no comorbid diagnosis. Intellectual functioning (FSIQ, VIQ, and NVIQ) did not differ between 22q11DS group with no comorbid diagnoses compared to those with psychosis-only diagnosis, nor those with ASD-only diagnosis (p > 0.9, n.s., for all comparisons). No significant differences in TASIT total score [F (3, 46) = 1.890, p = .15] and SRS total score [F (3, 42) = 2.894, p = .09] were observed as a function of ASD/psychosis comorbidity in 22q11DS. See Table 4 for means and standard deviations of TASIT, WASI, and SRS scores for each subgroup.

Table 4 22q11DS subgroup means and standard deviations for TASIT, SRS, and WASI-II IQ scores

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to directly examine the relationship between basic processes of social cognition, intellectual ability, and social behavior in individuals with 22q11DS compared to multiple primary idiopathic neuropsychiatric disorders simultaneously. We found that individuals with 22q11DS have more impaired social cognition than individuals with CHR and FEP, as well as healthy controls. While social cognition deficits in 22q11DS have been well established [9, 20, 21, 53], our results further illustrate that these deficits are more prominent than those observed for CHR and FEP groups across the age range, even after adjusting for global intellectual function. Additionally, despite group differences in overall intellectual functioning, 22q11DS individuals did not demonstrate significant social cognition deficits compared to the ASD group after adjusting for global intellectual function. This suggests that the social impairments present within this syndrome are not wholly accounted for by broad deficits in cognitive functioning. 22q11DS individuals both with and without comorbid psychiatric conditions consistently scored lower across all subdomains of the TASIT, highlighting their deficits in understanding reciprocal social exchange, and picking up lies and sarcasm in tone of voice, facial expression, and other visual or conversational cues. It is likely that haploinsufficiency of genes within the 22q11.2 locus and associated emotional and behavioral challenges [25] contribute to the observed substantial social cognition deficits in this population [53].

Consistent with prior work [17, 54, 55], we also found that individuals with 22q11DS had lower overall intellectual functioning across both verbal and non-verbal domains in comparison to heathy controls, as well as idiopathic ASD. Our findings additionally indicate that 22q11DS patients are impaired on overall intellectual functioning relative to idiopathic CHR and FEP. Regardless of the range of intellectual functioning, however, higher IQ was associated with better social cognition performance across all groups.

Although 22q11DS participants consistently showed poorer social cognition relative to CHR, FEP, and healthy controls, impairments were also present in youth with FEP (although this effect was no longer significant after controlling for IQ). This is in contrast to CHR youth, who did not show deficits in social cognition performance compared to HC. This finding, albeit cross-sectional, suggests there may be a decline in social cognition from prodrome to onset of overt psychotic symptoms. Prior studies have revealed that adult patients with psychosis show significant social cognitive deficits, particularly in theory of mind and social perception domains [56,57,58,59]. We also found poorer social cognition performance in participants with ASD relative to HC participants, consistent with the core social interaction deficits characteristic of this group [60]. Additional comparisons indicated that social cognition performance was better in participants with idiopathic ASD relative to 22q11DS subgroups with and without comorbid ASD. This suggests that even though behaviorally defined neuropsychiatric disorders are characterized by poorer social cognition performance compared to healthy controls, individuals with 22q11DS (both with and without comorbid neuropsychiatric disorders) experience greater impairments in social cognition compared to individuals with these idiopathic conditions.

Apart from the role of broader cognitive deficits accounted for in the present study, there are several more nuanced aspects of neurocognition that play a crucial role in social cognition abilities such as working memory and executive functioning skills [61,62,63,64,65]. Specifically, working memory is crucial for the encoding and retention of important social cues such as facial expression that are necessary to navigate the social world adequately [63, 64]. Additionally, working memory deficits have been associated with poor overall social competence and peer rejection in youth [66]. Working memory has also been strongly associated with the development of theory of mind skills through childhood and adolescence [67], and would hence be expected to influence performance on social cognition measures such as the TASIT. While working memory deficits are of particular concern in 22q11DS [5, 68,69,70], impairments in this domain are often seen in CHR [71,72,73], FEP [74,75,76], and ASD [77,78,79] as well. Given these findings, the working memory deficits characteristic of each of these neuropsychiatric conditions may play a role in the deficits in social cognition performance observed. Since working memory skills and fluid intelligence are highly correlated [80,81,82,83], it is possible that the observed relationships between broader intellectual functioning and social cognition performance across our patient groups are mediated by deficits in the working memory domain. Hence, further research into the specific role of working memory abilities in social cognition performance across these developmental neuropsychiatric disorders is warranted.

Finally, parent/caregiver report of real-world social behavior revealed considerable variability across all clinical groups, but were within a more concentrated range for the ASD group. On average, however, parent/caregiver reports of social difficulties fell within the clinically elevated range across all patient groups. Even within the 22q11DS subgroups, parent/caregiver reports of social difficulties were clinically elevated for all subgroups with or without comorbidities. Despite this, we did not find a significant relationship between social cognition performance and real-world social behavior for any clinical group, although the findings were trending in the expected direction across all groups. While numerous studies have used ToM tasks and the SRS concurrently in order to broadly examine social outcomes in our specified clinical groups, few have directly assessed the relationship between the two types of measures [20, 48, 84]. A recent study in ASD adolescents found that combined poor performance on several social cognition tasks (ToM tasks, emotion-recognition tasks, and anthropomorphic animations) was collectively associated with elevated SRS scores [85]. Some ASD studies have further found that parent/caregiver report of real-world social behaviors are better measures of treatment-related changes in social outcomes than are social cognition tasks, which suggests that these tasks may be measuring more nuanced social cognition skills such as ToM or attribution of intentions compared to everyday reciprocal social behaviors assessed by the SRS [86, 87]. One study on 22q11DS found a significant inverse association, indicating poorer performance on a ToM task (anthropomorphic animations) was associated with higher SRS scores [20]. Specific to the relationship between TASIT and SRS, a prior study did not find a significant association between TASIT performance and SRS in 22q11DS [48] in line with our findings. One study in adults meeting CHR criteria found a significant association between higher TASIT scores and lower self-reported SRS scores [50]. Another study on adults with ASD found a significant association between higher TASIT scores and higher self-reported SRS scores [88]. Hence, findings in the literature of the association between TASIT and SRS seem mixed, and likely vary as a result of primary diagnosis, self- versus other-report, and maybe insight into the participants’ own social difficulties with increasing age. Several studies that have specifically examined the relationship between performance-based measures and parent/caregiver reports of attention, executive functioning, memory, internalizing, and externalizing behaviors in a variety of neurodevelopmental conditions have found limited associations between the two types of measure suggesting that laboratory-based performance measures and parent report may be assessing different aspects of the same cognitive domain [89,90,91,92,93]. Hence, it is possible that the types of social concerns endorsed on the SRS (e.g., eye contact, odd behaviors, preference for being alone, ritualistic behaviors) may not relate as closely as we originally hypothesized to the TASIT’s measurement of nuanced social cognition skills such as lie and sarcasm detection even though they are both measures of social functioning.

In sum, our findings emphasize further need for assessment and interventions focused on improvement of social perception and social functioning in individuals with 22q11DS. Due to the overt medical problems that require urgent attention, intervention for social skills is more frequently overlooked in 22q11DS. In a recent review of previous cross-sectional as well as longitudinal studies assessing the early clinical signs in 22q11DS in predicting psychosis co-morbidity [94], it was collectively found that poor social functioning was linked to elevated negative and positive symptoms of psychosis. It may be beneficial for individuals with 22q11DS to be routinely referred by clinicians for social and cognitive skills training. Evidence from previous studies in idiopathic psychosis [95] and ASD [96] suggests that psychosocial and social skills training show positive treatment effects for both groups even 3–4 months post-treatment on average, suggesting good generalizability of social skills treatment gains, and improved overall functioning and sense of well-being. Intervention models directed at improving psychosocial function in individuals with 22q11DS have previously been proposed, with one pilot study that found targeted training in the identification of non-verbal social cues and perspective taking is associated with significant gains in social cognitive abilities in adolescents with 22q11DS [97]. Adapting existing treatment models from idiopathic ASD and psychosis to better suit the cognitive needs of individuals with 22q11DS could improve social cognition, and thereby overall quality of life. We further suggest close monitoring and early social skills intervention for individuals at risk for psychosis (whether genetically or behaviorally defined), before they begin to experience the decline in functioning related to onset of psychotic symptoms. Our findings of an association between intellectual ability and social functioning emphasizes the importance of accounting for neurocognitive deficits in these social skills interventions and tailoring these existing treatment models for both the 22q11DS and FEP populations. Our findings also suggest involving parents/caregivers more actively in social skills intervention for these groups, as their reported concerns in this domain may be valuable information that is not necessarily always captured by our existing assessments of social cognition.

Limitations and conclusions

The main limitation of this study is that our data were collected cross-sectionally across different research projects pertaining to each clinical population, and thus we were only able to compare those measures available across all clinical groups (WASI-II, TASIT, SRS). Further research is needed to examine other aspects of neurocognition, such as working memory, that may influence social skills beyond broad intellectual abilities, as well as inclusion of more comprehensive measures of social cognition. Additionally, SRS data were missing for several participants across groups, which could have impacted the lack of association with TASIT performance in our study. Lastly, our ASD sample was small (N = 24), as this is part of a newer ongoing study (K99MH113820; Author AN). Hence, findings for the ASD group may be underpowered compared to the other groups.

In conclusion, our findings suggest that social impairments are associated with neurocognitive deficits across all clinical groups, but more pronounced in 22q11DS compared to idiopathic neuropsychiatric disorders. These results contribute to the understanding of the intellectual and social vulnerabilities of 22q11DS in comparison to idiopathic neuropsychiatric disorders. The inclusion of multiple comparison groups with these conditions was essential to explore the specificity of social cognition deficits in 22q11DS. Social cognition deficits—and their relationship to intellectual functioning—are important factors in the clinical management of individuals with 22q11DS. Development of targeted interventions tailored to address social and intellectual difficulties experienced by 22q11DS is crucial to preventing declines in overall functioning and improving quality of life.

Availability of data and materials

The data supporting the conclusions of this article are included within the article and its additional files.

Abbreviations

22q11DS:

22q11.2 deletion syndrome

CHR:

Clinical high-risk

FEP:

First-episode psychosis

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

HC:

Healthy controls

TASIT:

The Awareness of Social Inference Test

SRS:

Social Responsiveness Scale

IQ:

Intellectual functioning

WASI-II:

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-Second Edition

ToM:

Theory of mind

UCLA:

University of California, Los Angeles

CAPPS:

Center for the Assessment and Prevention of Prodromal States

SIPS:

Structured Interview for Prodromal States

ABBRC:

Adolescent Brain-Behavior Research Center

CART:

Center for Autism Research and Treatment

ADOS-2:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second Edition

FSIQ:

Full-Scale IQ

VIQ:

Verbal IQ

NVIQ:

Non-verbal IQ

SPSS:

Statistical Package for the Social Sciences

SES:

Socio-economic status

ANOVAs:

Analysis of variance

ANCOVA:

Analysis of covariance

References

  1. Goldenberg PC, Calkins ME, Richard J, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai E, Mitra N, et al. Computerized neurocognitive profile in young people with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome compared to youths with schizophrenia and at-risk for psychosis. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2012;159B(1):87–93. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. McDonald-McGinn DM, Sullivan KE, Marino B, Philip N, Swillen A, Vorstman JA, et al. 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2015;1:15071.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Digilio MC, Angioni A, De Santis M, Lombardo A, Giannotti A, Dallapiccola B, et al. Spectrum of clinical variability in familial deletion 22q11.2: from full manifestation to extremely mild clinical anomalies. Clin Genet. 2003;63(4):308–13. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1399-0004.2003.00049.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Bassett AS, Chow EW, Husted J, Weksberg R, Caluseriu O, Webb GD, et al. Clinical features of 78 adults with 22q11 deletion syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2005;138(4):307–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.30984.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Kiley-Brabeck K, Sobin C. Social skills and executive function deficits in children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Appl Neuropsychol. 2006;13(4):258–68. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1304_7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Swillen A, McDonald-McGinn D. Developmental trajectories in 22q11.2 deletion. Am J Med Genet C: Semin Med Genet. 2015;169(2):172–81. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.c.31435.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Zhao Y, Guo T, Fiksinski A, Breetvelt E, McDonald-McGinn DM, Crowley TB, et al. Variance of IQ is partially dependent on deletion type among 1,427 22q11.2 deletion syndrome subjects. Am J Med Genet A. 2018;176(10):2172–81. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.40359.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Cabaral MH, Beaton EA, Stoddard J, Simon TJ. Impaired multiple object tracking in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2012;4(1):6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Campbell L, McCabe K, Leadbeater K, Schall U, Loughland C, Rich D. Visual scanning of faces in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: attention to the mouth or the eyes? Psychiatry Res. 2010;177(1-2):211–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.06.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Bearden CE, Woodin MF, Wang PP, Moss E, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai E, et al. The neurocognitive phenotype of the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: selective deficit in visual-spatial memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2001;23(4):447–64. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.23.4.447.1228.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Chawner S, Doherty JL, Moss H, Niarchou M, Walters JTR, Owen MJ, et al. Childhood cognitive development in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: case-control study. Br J Psychiatry. 2017;211(4):223–30. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.116.195651.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Moss EM, Batshaw ML, Solot CB, Gerdes M, McDonald-McGinn DM, Driscoll DA, et al. Psychoeducational profile of the 22q11.2 microdeletion: a complex pattern. J Pediatr. 1999;134(2):193–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3476(99)70415-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Woodin M, Wang PP, Aleman D, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai E, Moss E. Neuropsychological profile of children and adolescents with the 22q11.2 microdeletion. Genet Med. 2001;3(1):34–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/00125817-200101000-00008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Vorstman JA, Morcus ME, Duijff SN, Klaassen PW, Heineman-de Boer JA, Beemer FA, et al. The 22q11.2 deletion in children: high rate of autistic disorders and early onset of psychotic symptoms. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45(9):1104–13. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000228131.56956.c1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Sobin C, Kiley-Brabeck K, Karayiorgou M. Associations between prepulse inhibition and executive visual attention in children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(6):553–62. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001609.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Norkett EM, Lincoln SH, Gonzalez-Heydrich J, D'Angelo EJ. Social cognitive impairment in 22q11 deletion syndrome: a review. Psychiatry Res. 2017;253:99–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.01.103.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. McCabe KL, Melville JL, Rich D, Strutt PA, Cooper G, Loughland CM, et al. Divergent patterns of social cognition performance in autism and 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS). J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(8):1926–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1742-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Glaser B, Debbane M, Ottet MC, Vuilleumier P, Zesiger P, Antonarakis SE, et al. Eye gaze during face processing in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(7):665–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.04.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Campbell LE, McCabe KL, Melville JL, Strutt PA, Schall U. Social cognition dysfunction in adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (velo-cardio-facial syndrome): relationship with executive functioning and social competence/functioning. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2015;59(9):845–59. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12183.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ho JS, Radoeva PD, Jalbrzikowski M, Chow C, Hopkins J, Tran WC, et al. Deficits in mental state attributions in individuals with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (velo-cardio-facial syndrome). Autism Res. 2012;5(6):407–18. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1252.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Jalbrzikowski M, Carter C, Senturk D, Chow C, Hopkins JM, Green MF, et al. Social cognition in 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome: relevance to psychosis? Schizophr Res. 2012;142(1-3):99–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.10.007.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Van Den Heuvel E, Jonkers E, Rombouts E, Manders E, Zink I, Swillen A. Exploratory study on cognitive abilities and social responsiveness in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS) and children with idiopathic intellectual disability (IID). Res Dev Disabil. 2018;81:89–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2018.04.026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Campbell LE, Stevens A, Daly E, Toal F, Azuma R, Karmiloff-Smith A, et al. A comparative study of cognition and brain anatomy between two neurodevelopmental disorders: 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(4):1034–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Gur RE, Yi JJ, McDonald-McGinn DM, Tang SX, Calkins ME, Whinna D, et al. Neurocognitive development in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: comparison with youth having developmental delay and medical comorbidities. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(11):1205–11. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2013.189.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Jonas RK, Montojo CA, Bearden CE. The 22q11.2 deletion syndrome as a window into complex neuropsychiatric disorders over the lifespan. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75(5):351–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.07.019.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Antshel KM, Aneja A, Strunge L, Peebles J, Fremont WP, Stallone K, et al. Autistic spectrum disorders in velo-cardio facial syndrome (22q11.2 deletion). J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(9):1776–86. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0308-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Niklasson L, Rasmussen P, Oskarsdottir S, Gillberg C. Neuropsychiatric disorders in the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Genet Med. 2001;3(1):79–84. https://doi.org/10.1097/00125817-200101000-00017.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Niklasson L, Rasmussen P, Oskarsdottir S, Gillberg C. Autism, ADHD, mental retardation and behavior problems in 100 individuals with 22q11 deletion syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2009;30(4):763–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2008.10.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Schneider M, Debbane M, Bassett AS, Chow EW, Fung WL, van den Bree M, et al. Psychiatric disorders from childhood to adulthood in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: results from the International Consortium on Brain and Behavior in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(6):627–39. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070864.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Jolin EM, Weller RA, Weller EB. Occurrence of affective disorders compared to other psychiatric disorders in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Affect Disord. 2012;136(3):222–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.025.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Marshall CR, Howrigan DP, Merico D, Thiruvahindrapuram B, Wu W, Greer DS, et al. Contribution of copy number variants to schizophrenia from a genome-wide study of 41,321 subjects. Nat Genet. 2017;49(1):27–35. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3725.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Cotter J, Granger K, Backx R, Hobbs M, Looi CY, Barnett JH. Social cognitive dysfunction as a clinical marker: a systematic review of meta-analyses across 30 clinical conditions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;84:92–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Debbane M, Glaser B, David MK, Feinstein C, Eliez S. Psychotic symptoms in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: neuropsychological and behavioral implications. Schizophr Res. 2006;84(2-3):187–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.01.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Keshavan MS, Kaneko Y. Secondary psychoses: an update. World Psychiatry. 2013;12(1):4–15. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Casanova MF, Casanova EL, Frye RE, Baeza-Velasco C, LaSalle JM, Hagerman RJ, et al. Editorial: secondary vs. idiopathic autism. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:297. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00297.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. McDonald S, Bornhofen C, Shum D, Long E, Saunders C, Neulinger K. Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): a clinical test of social perception. Disabil Rehabil. 2006;28(24):1529–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638280600646185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. McDonald S, Flanagan S, Rollins J, Kinch J. TASIT: a new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2003;18(3):219–38. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001199-200305000-00001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Addington J, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, Cornblatt B, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, et al. North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study: a collaborative multisite approach to prodromal schizophrenia research. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33(3):665–72. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbl075.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Addington J, Cadenhead KS, Cornblatt BA, Mathalon DH, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, et al. North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS 2): overview and recruitment. Schizophr Res. 2012;142(1-3):77–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.09.012.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Addington J, Liu L, Buchy L, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, Cornblatt BA, et al. North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS 2): the prodromal symptoms. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2015;203(5):328–35. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000000290.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. McGlashan TH, Miller TJ, Woods SW, Hoffman RE, Davidson L. Instrument for the assessment of prodromal symptoms and states. In: Miller T, Mednick SA, McGlashan TH, Libiger J, Johannessen JO, editors. Early intervention in psychotic disorders. NATO science series (Series D: behavioural and social sciences): Springer; 2001. p. 91.

    Google Scholar 

  42. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders: patient edition; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Lord C, Rutter M, Di Lavore PC, Risi S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule. 2nd ed: Western Psychological Services; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. 2nd ed: The Psychological Corporation; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social responsiveness scale. 2nd ed: Western Psychological Services; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Genetic structure of reciprocal social behavior. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(12):2043–5. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.12.2043.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social responsiveness scale: Western Psychological Services; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Vangkilde A, Jepsen JR, Schmock H, Olesen C, Arnarsdottir S, Baare WF, et al. Associations between social cognition, skills, and function and subclinical negative and positive symptoms in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2016;8:42.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. D'Angelo EJ, Morelli N, Lincoln SH, Graber K, Tembulkar S, Gaudet A, et al. Social impairment and social language deficits in children and adolescents with and at risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2019;204:304–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2018.07.028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Glenthoj LB, Fagerlund B, Hjorthoj C, Jepsen JRM, Bak N, Kristensen TD, et al. Social cognition in patients at ultra-high risk for psychosis: what is the relation to social skills and functioning? Schizophr Res Cogn. 2016;5:21–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2016.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(6):655–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Bruni T. Test review: social responsiveness scale-second edition (SRS-2). J Psychoeduc Assess. 2014;32(4):365–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282913517525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Shashi V, Veerapandiyan A, Schoch K, Kwapil T, Keshavan M, Ip E, et al. Social skills and associated psychopathology in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: implications for interventions. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2012;56(9):865–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01477.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Kates WR, Antshel KM, Fremont WP, Shprintzen RJ, Strunge LA, Burnette CP, et al. Comparing phenotypes in patients with idiopathic autism to patients with velocardiofacial syndrome (22q11 DS) with and without autism. Am J Med Genet A. 2007;143A(22):2642–50. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.32012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Weinberger R, Yi J, Calkins M, Guri Y, McDonald-McGinn DM, Emanuel BS, et al. Neurocognitive profile in psychotic versus nonpsychotic individuals with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;26(10):1610–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.08.003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Bora E, Eryavuz A, Kayahan B, Sungu G, Veznedaroglu B. Social functioning, theory of mind and neurocognition in outpatients with schizophrenia; mental state decoding may be a better predictor of social functioning than mental state reasoning. Psychiatry Res. 2006;145(2-3):95–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2009;109(1-3):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2008.12.020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Savla GN, Vella L, Armstrong CC, Penn DL, Twamley EW. Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(5):979–92. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs080.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(10):620–31. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn4005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. DSM-V. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.): Author: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Meyer ML, Lieberman MD. Social working memory: neurocognitive networks and directions for future research. Front Psychol. 2012;3:571.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Thornton MA, Conway AR. Working memory for social information: chunking or domain-specific buffer? Neuroimage. 2013;70:233–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. de Wilde A, Koot HM, van Lier PA. Developmental links between children’s working memory and their social relations with teachers and peers in the early school years. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44(1):19–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0053-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Gambarota F, Sessa P. Visual working memory for faces and facial expressions as a useful “tool” for understanding social and affective cognition. Front Psychol. 2019;10:2392. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02392.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Madjar N, Chubarov E, Zalsman G, Weiser M, Shoval G. Social skills, executive functioning and social engagement. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2019;17:100137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2019.100137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. McQuade JD, Murray-Close D, Shoulberg EK, Hoza B. Working memory and social functioning in children. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;115(3):422–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Carlson SM, Koenig MA, Harms MB. Theory of mind. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2013;4(4):391–402. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Sobin C, Kiley-Brabeck K, Daniels S, Khuri J, Taylor L, Blundell M, et al. Neuropsychological characteristics of children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome: a descriptive analysis. Child Neuropsychol. 2005;11(1):39–53. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040590911167.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Wong LM, Riggins T, Harvey D, Cabaral M, Simon TJ. Children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome exhibit impaired spatial working memory. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2014;119(2):115–32. https://doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-119.2.115.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Shapiro HM, Tassone F, Choudhary NS, Simon TJ. The development of cognitive control in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Front Psychol. 2014;5:566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Fusar-Poli P, Deste G, Smieskova R, Barlati S, Yung AR, Howes O, et al. Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(6):562–71. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Pflueger MO, Gschwandtner U, Stieglitz RD, Riecher-Rossler A. Neuropsychological deficits in individuals with an at risk mental state for psychosis - working memory as a potential trait marker. Schizophr Res. 2007;97(1-3):14–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Fusar-Poli P, Broome MR, Matthiasson P, Woolley JB, Johns LC, Tabraham P, et al. Spatial working memory in individuals at high risk for psychosis: longitudinal fMRI study. Schizophr Res. 2010;123(1):45–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.06.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Zanello A, Curtis L, Badan Ba M, Merlo MC. Working memory impairments in first-episode psychosis and chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2009;165(1-2):10–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.10.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Pflueger MO, Calabrese P, Studerus E, Zimmermann R, Gschwandtner U, Borgwardt S, et al. The neuropsychology of emerging psychosis and the role of working memory in episodic memory encoding. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2018;11:157–68. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S149425.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Gonzalez-Ortega I, de Los MV, Echeburua E, Mezo M, Besga A, Ruiz de Azua S, et al. Working memory as a predictor of negative symptoms and functional outcome in first episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2013;206(1):8–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.08.025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Steele SD, Minshew NJ, Luna B, Sweeney JA. Spatial working memory deficits in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(4):605–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0202-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Barendse EM, Hendriks MP, Jansen JF, Backes WH, Hofman PA, Thoonen G, et al. Working memory deficits in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: neuropsychological and neuroimaging correlates. J Neurodev Disord. 2013;5(1):14. https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-5-14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Wang Y, Zhang YB, Liu LL, Cui JF, Wang J, Shum DH, et al. A Meta-analysis of working memory impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychol Rev. 2017;27(1):46–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-016-9336-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Ye T, Li P, Zhang Q, Gu Q, Lu X, Gao Z, et al. Relation between working memory capacity of biological movements and fluid intelligence. Front Psychol. 2019;10:2313. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02313.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(19):6829–33. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0801268105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Unsworth N, Fukuda K, Awh E, Vogel EK. Working memory and fluid intelligence: capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval. Cogn Psychol. 2014;71:1–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.01.003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Dehn MJ. How working memory enables fluid reasoning. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2017;6(3):245–7. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2017.1317490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Jones CRG, Simonoff E, Baird G, Pickles A, Marsden AJS, Tregay J, et al. The association between theory of mind, executive function, and the symptoms of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2018;11(1):95–109. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1873.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Baron-Cohen S, O'Riordan M, Stone V, Jones R, Plaisted K. Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29(5):407–18. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023035012436.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Friedrich EV, Sivanathan A, Lim T, Suttie N, Louchart S, Pillen S, et al. An effective neurofeedback intervention to improve social interactions in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(12):4084–100. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2523-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Soorya LV, Siper PM, Beck T, Soffes S, Halpern D, Gorenstein M, et al. Randomized comparative trial of a social cognitive skills group for children with autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54(3):208–16 e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.12.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Cole EJ, Slocombe KE, Barraclough NE. Abilities to explicitly and implicitly infer intentions from actions in adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(5):1712–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3425-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Krivitzky L, Bosenbark DD, Ichord R, Jastrzab L, Billinghurst L. Brief report: relationship between performance testing and parent report of attention and executive functioning profiles in children following perinatal arterial ischemic stroke. Child Neuropsychol. 2019;25(8):1116–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2019.1588957.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Gross AC, Deling LA, Wozniak JR, Boys CJ. Objective measures of executive functioning are highly discrepant with parent-report in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2015;21(4):531–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2014.911271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Ten Eycke KD, Dewey D. [Formula: see text]Parent-report and performance-based measures of executive function assess different constructs. Child Neuropsychol. 2016;22(8):889–906. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2015.1065961.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Toplak ME, West RF, Stanovich KE. Practitioner review: do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2013;54(2):131–43. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Limbers C, Young D, Jernigan S, Bryant W, Stephen M. Comparison between objective measures and parental behavioral rating scales of memory and attention in pediatric endocrinology patients. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2017;6(2):172–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2016.1152892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Van L, Boot E, Bassett AS. Update on the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and its relevance to schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2017;30(3):191–6. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Turner DT, McGlanaghy E, Cuijpers P, van der Gaag M, Karyotaki E, MacBeth A. A meta-analysis of social skills training and related interventions for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(3):475–91. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Hotton M, Coles S. The effectiveness of social skills training groups for individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;3(1):68–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-015-0066-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Shashi V, Harrell W, Eack S, Sanders C, McConkie-Rosell A, Keshavan MS, et al. Social cognitive training in adolescents with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: feasibility and preliminary effects of the intervention. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2015;59(10):902–13. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12192.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Special thanks to the participating families and staff who contributed toward data collection.

Funding

This work was supported by NIH-NIMH National Institute of Mental Health (1U54EB020403-01), (U01MH101719), (1R01MH100900-01), (R01 MH085953), NIMH K99MH113820, The Simons Foundation (Explorer Award to CEB), IMHRO/One Mind, and Brain-Behavior Research Foundation (NARSAD).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RJ, AN, and CEB made substantial contributions to study conception, analyses, and drafting of the manuscript. AL and AE contributed towards data analyses. LK and JZ oversaw recruitment and behavioral assessment. All authors participated in manuscript revisions and have given final approval of this version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aarti Nair.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All data presented in this study were collected upon approval for the University of California Los Angeles’s Institutional Review Board.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Supplementary Table 1

. Group means and standard deviations for SRS total and subscale T-scores. Supplementary Table 2. Spearman correlations between TASIT total score and SRS subscale scores. Supplementary Table 3. ASD and 22q11DS subgroup means and standard deviations for TASIT, SRS, and WASI-II IQ scores

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jalal, R., Nair, A., Lin, A. et al. Social cognition in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and idiopathic developmental neuropsychiatric disorders. J Neurodevelop Disord 13, 15 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-021-09363-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-021-09363-4

Keywords