Skip to main content

Developmental milestones and daily living skills in individuals with Angelman syndrome

Abstract

Background

Angelman syndrome (AS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder associated with severe global developmental delay. However, the ages at which different developmental skills are achieved in these individuals remain unclear. We seek to determine the probability and the age of acquisition of specific developmental milestones and daily living skills in individuals with AS across the different molecular subtypes, viz. class I deletion, class II deletion, uniparental disomy, imprinting defect, and UBE3A variants.

Methods

Caregivers participating in a longitudinal multicenter Angelman Syndrome Natural History Study completed a questionnaire regarding the age at which their children achieved specific developmental milestones and daily living skills. The Cox Proportional Hazard model was applied to analyze differences in the probability of achievement of skills at various ages among five molecular subtypes of AS.

Results

Almost all individuals, regardless of molecular subtype, were able to walk with support by five years of age. By age 15, those with a deletion had at least a 50% probability of acquiring 17 out of 30 skills compared to 25 out of 30 skills among those without a deletion. Overall, fine and gross motor skills such as holding and reaching for small objects, sitting, and walking with support were achieved within a fairly narrow range of ages, while toileting, feeding, and hygiene skills tend to have greater variability in the ages at which these skills were achieved. Those without a deletion had a higher probability (25–92%) of achieving daily living skills such as independently toileting and dressing compared to those with a deletion (0–13%). Across all molecular subtypes, there was a low probability of achieving independence in bathing and brushing teeth.

Conclusion

Individuals with AS without a deletion are more likely to achieve developmental milestones and daily living skills at an earlier age than those with a deletion. Many individuals with AS are unable to achieve daily living skills necessary for independent self-care.

Background

Angelman syndrome (AS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by severe intellectual disability, minimal or absent speech, ataxia, epilepsy, and sleep disturbances [1,2,3]. The prevalence of AS is approximately 1 in 22,000 to 1 in 52,000 [4,5,6,7,8]. AS is caused by lack of expression in neurons of the maternally-inherited copy of the ubiquitin-protein ligase E3A gene (UBE3A) on chromosome 15q11.2 [9,10,11] due to one of four mechanisms: deletion on the maternal copy of chromosome 15q11.2q13.1 (approximately 65–70% of individuals with AS), paternal uniparental disomy (UPD) (approximately 10%), an imprinting defect (ImpD) that results in the maternal allele being silenced in neurons (approximately 5–10%), and a pathogenic variant in the maternally-inherited UBE3A allele (about approximately 10–15%). The deletion subtypes can be further classified into: (a) class I with a 5.9 Mb deletion (40%), (b) class II with a 5.0 Mb deletion (50–55%), and (c) atypical deletions, which are smaller than class II or larger than class I (5–10%) [1, 12,13,14]. Those with a deletion are classified as “deletion-positive” while those with all other molecular subtypes are classified as “deletion-negative”.

Previous studies have found that individuals with AS are significantly delayed across all domains of development, but they do make slow developmental gains over time [15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23]. The severity of the developmental delay varies by molecular subtype; those with a deletion have more severe developmental delay than those without a deletion [16, 17, 19,20,21,22,23,24,25]..

The likelihood and the rate of acquiring the various developmental milestones are dependent on the molecular subtype of the child. Reports on the ages at which specific milestones are achieved are limited and have focused mainly on gross motor skills [16, 22, 26]. For example, Lossie et al. found that individuals with a deletion achieve motor milestones at a later age compared to those without deletion (sitting: 1.3 years versus 0.7–1.0 years; walking independently: 4.6 years versus 2.5–2.9 years) [26].

Understanding developmental milestones in AS is important both for clinical management and to determine the efficacy of future potential treatments. These milestones can help clinicians provide caregivers with more accurate evidence-based prognoses of developmental outcomes based on molecular subtype. If a child with AS is progressing more slowly than expected, clinicians should consider potential medical complications that might be affecting the child’s development (such as subclinical seizures or non-convulsive status epilepticus) or a second diagnosis that might explain the deviation from the anticipated developmental trajectory. More realistic developmental goals can also be set by the child’s educational intervention team using these milestones. Attainment of milestones earlier than expected in clinical trials of compounds that target neurodevelopment would suggest that the investigational product might be effective.

Using data from a large-scale longitudinal multicenter natural history study, we sought to determine the ages at which various developmental milestones and daily living skills were acquired in individuals with AS and how age of acquisition varies across different molecular etiologies.

Methods

Study population

Participants in this study were from the AS Natural History study (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT00296764). Inclusion criteria included: a confirmed molecular diagnosis of AS, absence of other co-morbid developmental disorders (e.g., severe prematurity or an additional genetic diagnosis), and age between one day and 60 years. We excluded individuals with a deletion of an atypical or unknown size, and individuals without developmental milestone data. Participants were evaluated annually at one of six study sites: Rady Children’s Hospital San Diego, Texas Children’s Hospital, Greenwood Genetic Center, Boston Children’s Hospital, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, and Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center. The study was approved by the institutional review boards at each study site.

Measure

At each study visit, parents completed a clinician-developed questionnaire [supplementary material] on whether their child had acquired specific developmental milestones in the domains of gross and fine motor development, and receptive and expressive language, and if so, the ages at which those skills were acquired. However, in this study, only a subset of developmental milestones was analyzed. The specific gross motor, fine motor, and expressive language skills that were analyzed were those that met the following criteria: (a) were meaningful to the parents, (b) impacted quality of life and daily functioning, (c) representative of development in each domain and (d) the study team believed that parents could recall accurately. Gross motor milestones included skills such as sitting unsupported, commando crawl, four-point crawl, pulling to stand, walking with support, and walking independently. Fine motor milestones included skills such as reaching for an object, holding an object, transferring an object from one hand to another, and using a pincer grasp. Expressive language included skills such as gesturing or pointing for wants, use of manual signs (including American Sign Language and enhanced natural gestures), and use of single words. Questions on daily living skills (toileting, dressing, washing hands, bathing, brushing teeth, and feeding) were introduced later in the study, so only a subset of the sample answered these questions.

Data cleaning

Analysis of a given skill by a participant was excluded if: (i) acquisition of the skill was marked “unknown”; (ii) participants were classified as having acquired the skill, but the age of acquisition was missing or unknown; (iii) the age of skill acquisition was greater than the chronological age at the visit; and (iv) age of skill acquisition was inconsistently reported at different study visits. Application of these data cleaning rules resulted in little data loss (less than 10% for most skills).

Statistical analysis

The Cox Proportional Hazard (CPH) Model was used to compare the probability that individuals with AS would achieve selected developmental milestones and daily living skills across five molecular subtypes, i.e., class I deletion, class II deletion, UPD, ImpD, and UBE3A pathogenic variants. The CPH is a regression model well suited to analyzing data where time to an event is the outcome of interest. When an individual fails to achieve the event during the observation window, the data are considered “censored”, indicating that it is possible that the participant could achieve the skill in the future beyond the temporal observation window of this study.

Cumulative hazard curves were created to visually compare the probability of achieving the 30 selected developmental milestones and daily living skills (six gross motor, four fine motor, three expressive language, three toileting, five dressing, six hygiene, and three feeding skills). The hazard rate indicates the probability of achieving a given skill at a particular age. The hazard rate for participants with a class I deletion, UPD, ImpD, or a UBE3A pathogenic variant was compared to the reference hazard rate of the participants with the most common molecular subtype in our sample (i.e., class II deletion), which had the largest sample size, by testing the ratio of the hazard rates for significance. The ratio of two hazard rates is called the hazard ratio. Hazard ratios greater than 1 indicate a higher probability of achieving the skill compared to participants with a class II deletion, whereas hazard ratios less than 1 indicate a lower probability of achieving the skill compared to participants with the reference subtype. Hazard ratios with p-values less than 0.05 were considered significant.

The ages at which there was a 5% and a 95% probability of acquiring a given skill were identified as a way to capture the range in the ages at which each skill was likely to be achieved. Graphical results were plotted for participants within each molecular subtype, truncated at 15 years of age because little to no change in development was observed beyond this point. Moreover, approximately 90% of the observations included in these analyses were from individuals younger than 15 years of age.

An assumption of the CPH model is that the hazard ratio is constant across time. If the assumption holds, the relative risk of an event is constant over time. The cox.zph R function was used to test for statistically significant violations of the proportional hazard assumption. In addition, recall duration was computed as the time interval between when the skill was achieved and when it was first reported and included as a model covariate, with significant results indicating possible recall bias. Sex was also included as a covariate to determine whether there were any differences between males and females in the probability of skill acquisition. All significance tests were evaluated using an alpha level of p < 0.05.

Results

Sample characteristics

Caregivers of 261 participants (Table 1) completed the developmental milestones questionnaire. Twenty-eight percent of the participants had a class I deletion, 40% had a class II deletion, 13% had UBE3A pathogenic variants, 8% had ImpD, and 11% had UPD. All the UBE3A variants were loss-of-function variants [27]. The number of visits per participant ranged from one to nine (Mean: 3.7, SD: 2.3). Age at baseline visit ranged from 0.4 years to 40.6 years (Mean: 5.6, SD: 5.3). There were no significant differences in the number of visits, age at the first (baseline) visit, or age at final visit among participants with different molecular subtypes. A subset of 211 participants completed the daily living skills questionnaire and on average, they were slightly older at baseline (Mean: 6.1, SD: 5.7) and had more visits (Mean: 4.1, SD: 2.3) compared to those who did not complete this questionnaire.

Table 1 Sample Characteristics

Probability of achieving different developmental milestones

The probability of an individual achieving a specific skill at a given age for various developmental milestones and daily living skills is depicted in Fig. 1. We found that some developmental milestones in the fine and gross motor domains were more likely to be achieved compared to other types of skills. For example, almost all individuals, regardless of molecular subtype, were able to walk with support by age five years. In contrast, for some molecular subtypes certain daily living skills were more difficult to achieve (i.e., probabilities remained low across the age range and were achieved by fewer than 95% of the subgroup at 15 years of age). For example, the probability that an individual can brush his/her teeth with assistance by 15 years of age was 13% for participants with class I deletion compared to 85% for those with a UBE3A pathogenic variant. Other daily living skills such as brushing teeth independently and bathing independently had a low probability of achievement by age 15 across molecular subtypes (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Probability of Skill Achievement for Critical Developmental Milestones and Daily Living Skills by Molecular Subtype

Note. Dashed lines indicate point on the curve associated with a 50% probability of achieving the skill. Higher probabilities indicate that more individuals with AS are expected to achieve the skill. Steeper curves indicate that individuals tend to achieve the skill in a relatively narrow age range whereas flatter curves indicate a wide range of ages where individuals achieve the skill

Figures 2, 3, 4, 5 and 6 illustrate the ages at which there is a 5%, 50%, and 95% probability of achieving the selected developmental milestones and daily living skills among participants of each molecular subtype. For each skill with relevant comparative data, we have also indicated the age at which at least 75% of children in the general population achieve the milestone [28]. The probability of achieving these skills was found to be highly dependent on molecular subtype. By age 15, those with a deletion had at least a 50% probability of acquiring 17 out of 30 skills (Figs. 2 & 3) compared to 25 out of 30 skills among those with deletion-negative subtypes (Figs. 4, 5 and 6). Compared to deletion-negative participants, deletion-positive individuals had a lower probability of achieving self-care skills such as brushing teeth independently and hand washing independently by age 15. For example, the probability of independent hand washing was 91% by age 15 for those with UBE3A pathogenic variants (Fig. 4), 74% for those with UPD (Fig. 5), but only 13% for those with a class I deletion (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Probability of Skill Achievement in Class I Deletion

From left to right, the first black triangle represents a probability of 0.05, the beginning of the shaded gray bar represents a probability of 0.25, the black circle represents a probability of 0.50, the end of shaded gray bar on the right represents a probability of 0.75, and a second black triangle on the right represents a probability of 0.95. The red X indicates the age at which the milestone is achieved by ≥ 75% children in the general population. For the feeding skill of independent utensil use ("Utensils Independ") the first red X indicates use of a spoon, the second red X indicates use of a fork. The axis on the right indicates the probability of skill achievement. This value is either 0.95, or the probability of skill achievement at 15 years of age in cases where the probability did not reach 0.95

Fig. 3
figure 3

Probability of Skill Achievement in Class II Deletion

From left to right, the first black triangle represents a probability of 0.05, the beginning of the shaded gray bar represents a probability of 0.25, the black circle represents a probability of 0.50, the end of shaded gray bar on the right represents a probability of 0.75, and a second black triangle on the right represents a probability of 0.95. The red X indicates the age at which the milestone is achieved by ≥ 75% children in the general populationFor the feeding skill of independent utensil use ("Utensils Independ") the first red X indicates use of a spoon, the second red X indicates use of a fork. The axis on the right indicates the probability of skill achievement. This value is either 0.95, or the probability of skill achievement at 15 years of age in cases where the probability did not reach 0.95

Fig. 4
figure 4

Probability of Skill Achievement in UBE3A Mutation

From left to right, the first black triangle represents a probability of 0.05, the beginning of the shaded gray bar represents a probability of 0.25, the black circle represents a probability of 0.50, the end of shaded gray bar on the right represents a probability of 0.75, and a second black triangle on the right represents a probability of 0.95. The red X indicates the age at which the milestone is achieved by ≥ 75% children in the general populationFor the feeding skill of independent utensil use ("Utensils Independ") the first red X indicates use of a spoon, the second red X indicates use of a fork. The axis on the right indicates the probability of skill achievement. This value is either 0.95, or the probability of skill achievement at 15 years of age in cases where the probability did not reach 0.95

Fig. 5
figure 5

Probability of Skill Achievement in Uniparental Disomy

From left to right, the first black triangle represents a probability of 0.05, the beginning of the shaded gray bar represents a probability of 0.25, the black circle represents a probability of 0.50, the end of shaded gray bar on the right represents a probability of 0.75, and a second black triangle on the right represents a probability of 0.95. The red X indicates the age at which the milestone is achieved by ≥ 75% children in the general populationFor the feeding skill of independent utensil use ("Utensils Independ") the first red X indicates use of a spoon, the second red X indicates use of a fork. The axis on the right indicates the probability of skill achievement. This value is either 0.95, or the probability of skill achievement at 15 years of age in cases where the probability did not reach 0.95

Fig. 6
figure 6

Probability of Skill Achievement in Imprinting Defect

From left to right, the first black triangle represents a probability of 0.05, the beginning of the shaded gray bar represents a probability of 0.25, the black circle represents a probability of 0.50, the end of shaded gray bar on the right represents a probability of 0.75, and a second black triangle on the right represents a probability of 0.95. The red X indicates the age at which the milestone is achieved by ≥ 75% children in the general populationFor the feeding skill of independent utensil use ("Utensils Independ") the first red X indicates use of a spoon, the second red X indicates use of a fork. The axis on the right indicates the probability of skill achievement. This value is either 0.95, or the probability of skill achievement at 15 years of age in cases where the probability did not reach 0.95

Differences between molecular subtypes in the probability of skill acquisition were tested for significance using hazard ratios. Table 2 provides hazard ratios for the selected developmental milestones and daily living skills. The ratios comparing the class I and class II deletion groups were only significant for two out of the 30 skills (“putting on some clothes” [hazard ratio = 0.40, p < 0.05] and “washing hands with assistance” [hazard ratio = 0.37, p < 0.01]).

Table 2 Hazard Ratios for Developmental Skills by Molecular Subtype

Likewise, when comparing deletion negative individuals, results from Table 2 suggest that UBE3A and ImpD subtypes have a more similar developmental trajectory, developing most skills earlier than class II deletion (significant in 23 and 21 out of 29 testable skills respectively), while the developmental trajectory for UPD was only significantly different from the class II deletion subtype in 14 out of 29 testable skills. Overall, hazard ratios for deletion-negative subtypes were statistically significantly greater than 1 in multiple developmental domains, indicating that individuals without deletion had a higher probability of achieving these skills than individuals with a class II deletion. Although deletion-positive individuals are unlikely to achieve many daily living skills, deletion-negative individuals are likely to eventually develop some of these skills (e.g., washing hands with assistance and using utensils).

As noted in Table 2, there was evidence that the proportional hazards assumption was violated for all six gross motor skills and one fine motor skill (“hold object”). Further analysis indicated that the hazard ratio comparing class II deletion to ImpD was most often driving significant findings. Time-dependent coefficients were examined by analyzing changes in the slope of the Cox regression coefficient over time to determine appropriate time intervals. Findings indicated that the hazard ratios tend to be higher and more often significant compared to the values provided in Table 2, until approximately the age at which the ImpD subtype reached a 50% probability of acquiring the skill. After that age, the hazard rates for ImpD become more similar to those of class II deletion (i.e., hazard ratios approached 1).

Sex was only a statistically significant covariate for toileting skills (p < 0.05, model results not shown), such that females had a higher probability of achieving toileting skills compared to males at each age.

Out of 30 skills, recall time was found to be a significant covariate only for “uses toilet without reminder” (p < 0.05, model results not shown), suggesting a possible recall bias. Caregivers were more likely to report a younger age of acquisition for this skill when there was a longer time interval between when this skill was acquired and when the caregiver answered this question on the questionnaire. However, the hazard ratios for the expanded model that included recall time (data not shown) were within the confidence intervals of the original model, indicating that the impact of potential recall bias was small and indistinguishable from sampling variability and therefore, the original model results were retained. Sample sizes for censored (i.e., failing to achieve milestone/skill within the observation window) and uncensored (i.e., achieving the milestone/skill within the observation window) cases are provided in Table 3.

Table 3 Number of Uncensored and Censored Individuals for Developmental Skills by Molecular Subtype

Age of acquisition for different milestones

The ages (in months) at which there is a 5%, 50%, and 95% probability of acquiring a given skill among participants with each molecular subtype are shown in Table 4 and Figs. 2, 3, 4, 5 and 6. Across all molecular subtypes, fine motor skills such as holding an object were achieved during the first 3.5 years of life while gross motor skills such as walking with support were achieved within the first five years of life. Skills such as pincer grasp showed greater variability in the ages at which they were likely to be achieved even within the same molecular subtype. For example, individuals with UPD and class II deletion were likely to achieve the pincer grasp fine motor skill between ages 10–180 months (a range of 170 months). Daily living skills such as toileting, feeding, and hygiene skills also tended to have substantial variability in the ages at which the skills were likely to be achieved across molecular subtypes.

Table 4 Ages in Months and Probability of Achieving Developmental Skills by Molecular Subtype

Deletion-positive individuals developed skills at a later age range than the deletion-negative individuals. Results in Table 4 also indicate a wider range of ages for skill acquisition among deletion-positive individuals compared to deletion-negative individuals for most skills with the exception of “Reaches for Object”, “Uses Pincer Grasp”, and “Feeds Self With Hands” where the age range of skill acquisition among those with UPD was comparable to that for those with a deletion.

Discussion

Using data from a sample of 261 individuals in the AS Natural History study, we examined the attainment of developmental milestones and daily living skills in individuals with AS. Although previous studies have demonstrated that individuals with AS have developmental delays [15, 16, 21, 22], this is the first study to examine the probability and age at which a range of developmental milestones and daily living skills are acquired among individuals with AS due to different molecular etiologies.

We found significant variability in the probability of achieving different skills at various ages across AS subtypes. Of note, some “pre-walking” skills such as commando crawl and four-point crawl were never achieved by some of these individuals despite their ability to achieve higher-level skills such as walking with support. Overall, fine and gross motor skills such as holding and reaching for small objects, sitting, and walking with support had a higher probability of being achieved within a fairly narrow range of ages. On the other hand, many daily living skills such as toileting and feeding, had a lower probability of being acquired and exhibited greater variability in the age at which they were acquired. In addition, independence in performing some daily living skills such as brushing teeth, bathing, and dressing themselves fully were found to be very challenging, with only a small minority of the participants achieving these skills, especially among deletion-positive individuals. Factors such as seizures, access to early intervention services, earlier age of diagnosis, and the availability of new disease-modifying therapies may affect the age at which these milestones are achieved and should therefore be re-examined in future studies.

Additionally, consistent with prior literature, deletion-positive individuals were more delayed (i.e., achieved skills at older ages) and typically had a wider range in age of skill acquisition compared to non-deletion individuals [15, 16, 24, 26]. Deletion-positive individuals also achieved fewer self-care skills such as toileting, using utensils, and washing hands compared to non-deletion individuals. Among deletion-positive individuals, there was no difference in developmental profile of class I and class II deletions for most skills. The developmental profiles of individuals with UBE3A pathogenic variants and ImpD were also quite similar for most skills. The developmental profiles of individuals with UPD generally fell in between individuals with class II deletions and UBE3A pathogenic variants. These findings are not consistent with prior studies, which have found that individuals with UPD and ImpD have similar developmental profiles and are considered to be least affected developmentally [26]. Given the small sample sizes, the differences between the non-deletion molecular subtypes should be interpreted with caution.

Similarly, it is important to further explore the violation of the proportional hazard model assumption, specifically when comparing the hazard rates of ImpD to class II deletion. This exploration will help determine whether this violation is an artifact of the small sample size or an indication that after a certain age, individuals with ImpD who have not yet attained specific gross and fine motor skills tend to have a developmental profile more similar to deletion-positive individuals for these skills.

Sex differences were only evident in toileting skills, with females demonstrating a higher probability of achieving these skills compared to males. These results are consistent with findings in the neurotypical population where girls have been noted to have better bladder control attributed to their shorter urethra and heightened bodily awareness compared to boys [29]. It is also possible that parents may be attempting to initiate toilet training earlier for girls than for boys [30].

The failure to achieve daily living skills creates significant functional impairment and limits the ability of individuals to function independently, consistent with needing lifelong support and assistance [31,32,33]. These functional impairments may be sources of stress for caregivers and affect family quality of life [34]. It is therefore critical that ongoing interventions target a variety of daily living skills. Daily living skills should be broken down into smaller, more achievable tasks that can then be taught using behavioral techniques such as shaping, chaining, and discrete trial instruction [35, 36]. For example, in terms of toileting, the results of the current study suggest that individuals with AS are more likely to use the toilet when placed there rather than doing so independently. As such, working on regular or scheduled toilet visits with positive reinforcement may be appropriate strategies when toilet training individuals with AS [37, 38].

There are important clinical implications arising from our study. Our study provides clinicians and families with a useful tool to help provide more accurate developmental prognosis for children with AS, to monitor the developmental progress of these children, and for setting appropriate therapeutic and educational goals. If a child fails to make adequate developmental progress with intensive therapies, ruling out other medical concerns (e.g., seizures, co-existing genetic disorders) is critical. In addition, given the level of delay in acquisition of skills, individuals with AS should receive intensive physical, occupational, and speech-language therapy beginning early in life and continuing through adulthood. Teaching of functional daily living skills should be an important component of Individualized Education Plans for children with AS.

Although this study has many strengths, including a relatively large sample size for a rare disorder, we acknowledge some notable limitations. The use of caregiver report to gather information on the acquisition of developmental data may not be reliable. Caregivers are more likely to accurately recall some milestones compared to others because they are more meaningful to them (e.g., walking independently versus reaching) [39, 40]. In addition, when caregivers were asked to recall when a specific skill was achieved with assistance, the definition of ‘with assistance’ was not provided and could have been interpreted differently by parents. Although we assessed the data collected for recall bias based on age of recall and found limited evidence, prior studies have found parental report to be less accurate than assessments administered and scored by clinicians [41]. The presence of seizures could influence the age of acquisition of developmental milestones. However, due to the limited sample size of individuals without seizures in each genotype, and the unknown seizure status of several participants, we were unable to assess the potential effect of seizures on the development of various skills. Additional limitations are the relatively small sample sizes for some of the molecular subtypes used to calculate hazard rates and ratios, and the failure of all participants to achieve some of these skills during the data collection timeframe for many skills (i.e., highly censored data). Although continued longitudinal tracking of individuals with AS may decrease the proportion of censored data, if some skills are only achieved by a very small proportion of the AS population, censored data are unavoidable.

There was some evidence of violation of the proportional hazard assumption for the gross and fine motor skills. However, this mainly affected the point estimates of the hazard ratios for the ImpD and class II deletion comparisons. Notably, the pattern of results for these skills was consistent with the pattern observed for the other skills that did not violate the proportional hazard assumption, namely, that the hazard ratios comparing ImpD to class II deletion were typically greater than 1, indicating individuals with ImpD tended to acquire skills at a faster rate compared to those with class II deletion. In addition, our study focused primarily on the ages at which developmental milestones and adaptive skills were acquired. We did not gather additional information on how individuals with AS were using these specific skills in their daily life, whether the ability to use these skills was influenced by sensory or behavior challenges or lack of motivation, or whether there was any loss of skills over time. Moreover, the expressive language domain focused only on the age of acquisition of gestures, manual signs, and verbal speech; it did not include information on the use of augmentative and alternative communication systems. Future studies with a larger sample should not only assess whether an individual has acquired a skill but also whether they are able to effectively use a skill in their daily life, and whether regression is seen in any of the skills (especially as individuals age). Predictors of variability in developmental skills within molecular subtypes such as age of diagnosis, age of access to services, and presence of seizures should also be examined to better inform clinical care.

Conclusions

Using data from the AS Natural History study, we analyzed the probability and age at which various developmental milestones and daily living skills in individuals with AS across molecular subtypes were acquired. Results demonstrate significant developmental delays and functional challenges in AS and suggest the need for intensive interventions beginning early in life.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the Linking Angelman and Dup15q Data for Expanded Research (LADDER) database https://laddertotreatment.org/

Abbreviations

AS:

Angelman syndrome

UPD:

Uniparental disomy

ImpD:

Imprinting defect

CPH:

Cox proportional hazard

References

  1. Bird LM. Angelman syndrome: review of clinical and molecular aspects. Appl Clin Genet. 2014;7:93–104.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Dagli AI, Mathews J, Williams CA. Angelman Syndrome. In: Adam MP, Feldman J, Mirzaa GM, Pagon RA, Wallace SE, Bean LJH, Stephens K,et al. editors. GeneReviews® Seattle (WA): University of Washington, Seattle; 1998 [updated 2021].

  3. Williams CA, Beaudet AL, Clayton-Smith J, Knoll JH, Kyllerman M, Laan LA, et al. Angelman syndrome 2005: updated consensus for diagnostic criteria. Am J Med Genet A. 2006;140(5):413–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Luk HM, Lo IF. Angelman syndrome in Hong Kong Chinese: A 20 years’ experience. Eur J Med Genet. 2016;59(6–7):315–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Mertz LG, Christensen R, Vogel I, Hertz JM, Nielsen KB, Gronskov K, et al. Angelman syndrome in Denmark. birth incidence, genetic findings, and age at diagnosis. Am J Med Genet A. 2013;161A(9):2197–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Oiglane-Shlik E, Talvik T, Zordania R, Poder H, Kahre T, Raukas E, et al. Prevalence of Angelman syndrome and Prader-Willi syndrome in Estonian children: sister syndromes not equally represented. Am J Med Genet A. 2006;140(18):1936–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Petersen MB, Brondum-Nielsen K, Hansen LK, Wulff K. Clinical, cytogenetic, and molecular diagnosis of Angelman syndrome: estimated prevalence rate in a Danish county. Am J Med Genet. 1995;60(3):261–2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Yakoreva M, Kahre T, Zordania R, Reinson K, Teek R, Tillmann V, et al. A retrospective analysis of the prevalence of imprinting disorders in Estonia from 1998 to 2016. Eur J Hum Genet. 2019;27(11):1649–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Kishino T, Lalande M, Wagstaff J. UBE3A/E6-AP mutations cause Angelman syndrome. Nat Genet. 1997;15(1):70–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Matsuura T, Sutcliffe JS, Fang P, Galjaard RJ, Jiang YH, Benton CS, et al. De novo truncating mutations in E6-AP ubiquitin-protein ligase gene (UBE3A) in Angelman syndrome. Nat Genet. 1997;15(1):74–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Dagli A, Buiting K, Williams CA. Molecular and Clinical Aspects of Angelman Syndrome. Mol Syndromol. 2012;2(3–5):100–12.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Clayton-Smith J, Laan L. Angelman syndrome: a review of the clinical and genetic aspects. J Med Genet. 2003;40(2):87–95.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Buiting K, Williams C, Horsthemke B. Angelman syndrome - insights into a rare neurogenetic disorder. Nat Rev Neurol. 2016;12(10):584–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Beygo J, Buiting K, Ramsden SC, Ellis R, Clayton-Smith J, Kanber D. Update of the EMQN/ACGS best practice guidelines for molecular analysis of Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes. Eur J Hum Genet. 2019;27(9):1326–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Mertz LG, Thaulov P, Trillingsgaard A, Christensen R, Vogel I, Hertz JM, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome in Angelman syndrome: genotype-phenotype correlations. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(7):1742–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Bindels-de Heus K, Mous SE, Ten Hooven-Radstaake M, van Iperen-Kolk BM, Navis C, Rietman AB, et al. An overview of health issues and development in a large clinical cohort of children with Angelman syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2020;182(1):53–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Gentile JK, Tan WH, Horowitz LT, Bacino CA, Skinner SA, Barbieri-Welge R, et al. A neurodevelopmental survey of Angelman syndrome with genotype-phenotype correlations. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31(7):592–601.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Peters SU, Goddard-Finegold J, Beaudet AL, Madduri N, Turcich M, Bacino CA. Cognitive and adaptive behavior profiles of children with Angelman syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2004;128(2):110–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Sadhwani A, Wheeler A, Gwaltney A, Peters SU, Barbieri-Welge RL, Horowitz LT, et al. Developmental Skills of Individuals with Angelman Syndrome Assessed Using the Bayley-III. J Autism Dev Disord. 2023;53(2):720–37.

  20. Keute M, Miller MT, Krishnan ML, Sadhwani A, Chamberlain S, Thibert RL, et al. Angelman syndrome genotypes manifest varying degrees of clinical severity and developmental impairment. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26(7):3625–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Brun Gasca C, Obiols JE, Bonillo A, Artigas J, Lorente I, Gabau E, Guitart M, Turk J. Adaptive behaviour in Angelman syndrome: its profile and relationship to age. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54(11):1024–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Roche L, Tones M, Cross M, Bellgard M, Heussler H. An Overview of the Adaptive Behaviour Profile in Young Children with Angelman Syndrome: Insights from the Global Angelman Syndrome Registry. Advances in Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2022;9:1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Gwaltney A, Potter SN, Peters SU, Barbieri-Welge RL, Horowitz LT, Noll LM et al. Adaptive Skills of Individuals with Angelman Syndrome Assessed Using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd Edition. J Autism Dev Disord. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-023-06090-8.

  24. Yang L, Shu X, Mao S, Wang Y, Du X, Zou C. Genotype-Phenotype Correlations in Angelman Syndrome. Genes (Basel). 2021;12(7):987.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Wheeler AC, Okoniewski KC, Wylie A, DeRamus M, Hiruma LS, Toth D, et al. Anxiety-associated and separation distress-associated behaviours in Angelman syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2019;63(10):1234–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Lossie AC, Whitney MM, Amidon D, Dong HJ, Chen P, Theriaque D, et al. Distinct phenotypes distinguish the molecular classes of Angelman syndrome. J Med Genet. 2001;38(12):834–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Weston KP, Gao X, Zhao J, Kim KS, Maloney SE, Gotoff J, Parikh S, Leu YC, Wu KP, Shinawi M, Steimel JP, Harrison JS, Yi JJ. Identification of disease-linked hyperactivating mutations in UBE3A through large-scale functional variant analysis. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):6809.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Zubler JM, Wiggins LD, Macias MM, Whitaker TM, Shaw JS, Squires JK, Pajek JA, Wolf RB, Slaughter KS, Broughton AS, Gerndt KL, Mlodoch BJ, Lipkin PH. Evidence-Informed Milestones for Developmental Surveillance Tools. Pediatrics. 2022;149(3):e2021052138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Joinson C, Heron J, von Gontard A, Butler U, Golding J, Emond A. Early childhood risk factors associated with daytime wetting and soiling in school-age children. J Pediatr Psychol. 2008;33(7):739–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Schum TR, Kolb TM, McAuliffe TL, Simms MD, Underhill RL, Lewis M. Sequential acquisition of toilet-training skills: a descriptive study of gender and age differences in normal children. Pediatrics. 2002;109(3):E48. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.109.3.e48.PMID:11875176.30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Grieco JC, Romero B, Flood E, Cabo R, Visootsak J. A Conceptual Model of Angelman Syndrome and Review of Relevant Clinical Outcomes Assessments (COAs). Patient. 2019;12(1):97–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Willgoss T, Cassater D, Connor S, Krishnan ML, Miller MT, Dias-Barbosa C, et al. Measuring What Matters to Individuals with Angelman Syndrome and Their Families: Development of a Patient-Centered Disease Concept Model. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2021;52(4):654–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Wheeler AC, Sacco P, Cabo R. Unmet clinical needs and burden in Angelman syndrome: a review of the literature. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2017;12(1):164.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Miodrag N, Peters S. Parent stress across molecular subtypes of children with Angelman syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2015;59(9):816–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. A Stabel. Daily Living Skills. In: Volkmar FR, editor. Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders. New York, NY: Springer. York: New; 2013. p. 839–40.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Summers J, Szatmari P. Using discrete trial instruction to teach children with Angelman syndrome. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. 2009;24(4):216–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Radstaake M, Didden R, Peters-Scheffers N, Moore DW, Anderson A, Curfs LM. Toilet training in individuals with Angelman syndrome: a case series. Dev Neurorehabil. 2014;17(4):243–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Didden R, Sikkema SP, Bosman IT, Duker PC, Curfs LM. Use of a modified Azrin-Foxx toilet training procedure with individuals with Angelman syndrome. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2001;14(1):64–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Treharne DA. Parental recall of children’s early development. Eur J Disord Commun. 1992;27(3):221–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Majnemer A, Rosenblatt B. Reliability of parental recall of developmental milestones. Pediatr Neurol. 1994;10(4):304–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Ozonoff S, Iosif AM, Young GS, Hepburn S, Thompson M, Colombi C, Cook IC, Werner E, Goldring S, Baguio F, Rogers SJ. Onset patterns in autism: correspondence between home video and parent report. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2011 Aug 1;50(8):796–806. 41. Zubler JM, Wiggins LD, Macias MM, Whitaker TM, Shaw JS, Squires JK, Pajek JA, Wolf RB, Slaughter KS, Broughton AS, Gerndt KL, Mlodoch BJ, Lipkin PH. Evidence-Informed Milestones for Developmental Surveillance Tools. Pediatrics. 2022;149(3):e2021052138.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We extend our deep appreciation to all the individuals with AS and families who participated in this longitudinal study and contributed to these data. We are also grateful to the study coordinators for recruiting participants for the study and collecting the data. We appreciate the financial support from Ionis Pharmaceuticals and Ovid therapeutics for statistical analyses and thank Miganush Stepanians, PhD, and Nicole LaVallee, PhD, from Prometrika for their statistical guidance in the initial analyses of these data. Ionis Pharmaceuticals and Ovid therapeutics had no role or influence in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of these data in any way. We thank Angela Gwaltney for her statistical assistance during the revision of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by NIH U54 RR019478 (awarded to Arthur L. Beaudet) from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR) and NIH U54 HD061222 (awarded to Alan Percy) from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), both components of the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization and Design: AS, WHT, SP, ACW, HM; Data Curation: AS, WHT, LMB, SP, CB, SS, LW, CE; Statistical analysis: SP; Drafting initial draft: AS, SP; Contribution to the figures and tables: AS, ACW, HM, SP, SNP. All authors critically reviewed and revised the manuscript for important intellectual content and approved the final manuscript as submitted.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anjali Sadhwani.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All six institutions’ Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the study. Caregivers and legal guardians of the participants provided informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors have no conflicts of interest relevant to this article to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sadhwani, A., Powers, S., Wheeler, A. et al. Developmental milestones and daily living skills in individuals with Angelman syndrome. J Neurodevelop Disord 16, 32 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-024-09548-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-024-09548-7

Keywords