Skip to main content

Event-related potentials to repeated speech in 9-month-old infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder

Abstract

Background

Atypical neural responses to repeated auditory and linguistic stimuli have been reported both in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and their first-degree relatives. Recent work suggests that the younger siblings of children with ASD have atypical event-related potentials (ERPs) to repeated tones at 9 months of age; however, the functional significance is unclear, and it is unknown whether this atypicality is also present in response to linguistic stimuli.

Methods

We analyzed ERPs to repetitive and deviant consonant-vowel stimuli at 9 months in 35 unaffected high-risk-for-autism (HRA) infant siblings of children with ASD and 45 low-risk control (LRC) infants. We examined a positive component, the P150, over frontal and central electrode sites and investigated the relationships between this component and later behavior.

Results

Over frontal electrodes, HRA infants had larger-amplitude ERPs to repetitions of the standard than LRC infants, whereas ERPs to the deviant did not differ between HRA and LRC infants. Furthermore, for HRA infants, the amplitude of ERPs to the standards was positively correlated with later language ability.

Conclusions

Our work suggests that atypical ERPs to repeated speech during infancy are a possible endophenotype of ASD but that this atypicality is associated with beneficial, rather than disordered, language development. Potential mechanisms driving these relationships and implications for development are discussed.

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) involves social and communicative impairments in addition to restricted interests and repetitive behaviors and is often accompanied by language impairment [1]. Infants with a family history of ASD exhibit subtle atypicalities in how they process and interact with the world during the 1st year of life [24], and while these infants have an increased risk for developing ASD and other language or behavioral impairments, the majority are not ultimately clinically impaired [57]. A better understanding of how this population processes their environment during infancy and how this relates to their later development can help to understand why these infants vary in developmental outcome. Here, we focus specifically on how infants with a family history of ASD process repeated speech sounds, as atypical neural processing of repeated auditory stimuli have previously been reported both in individuals with ASD and their first-degree relatives [813].

Event-related potentials (ERPs) have been used extensively to study auditory and linguistic processing in adults and children with ASD, often by measuring electrophysiological response to a repeated auditory stimulus (a ‘standard’). Individuals with ASD have been shown to exhibit diminished cortical evoked potentials to both linguistic [810] and nonlinguistic ([813], although see [14]) standard stimuli, which has often been interpreted as reflecting inefficient auditory encoding. In typically developing children, cortical evoked potentials become weaker with successive repetition of an auditory stimulus, and there is evidence that children with ASD do not show this progressive dampening of response [15, 16].

Atypical neural responses to auditory and linguistic stimuli, including responses to repetitive stimuli, are also evident in the unaffected relatives of individuals with ASD [12, 1719], suggesting that these may serve as familial risk markers of autism; however, these atypicalities can manifest differently in individuals with ASD than in their unaffected family members [12, 17]. For example, children with ASD show dampened evoked potentials to repeated auditory stimuli relative to typical controls, whereas their parents have been reported as having atypically large evoked potentials to these stimuli relative to typical adults [12]. No direct comparisons of neural responses to auditory repetition have been made between age- or behavior-matched groups of individuals with ASD and unaffected individuals with a family history of ASD, making interpretation of group differences difficult, and it is possible that group differences in neural response are due to group differences in behavioral characteristics (e.g., cognitive ability and language ability) rather than ASD specifically.

It also remains unclear at what point patterns of atypical auditory and linguistic processing begin developmentally and how this relates to later behavior, both in individuals who develop ASD and relatives who do not. ASD is highly heritable, and approximately 20% of infants with an older sibling with ASD (i.e., high-risk-for-autism infants, or HRA infants) will develop the disorder themselves [20]. In these infants who develop ASD, overt behavioral symptoms begin to emerge by around 12 months of age and are often sufficiently pronounced and stable enough to support a diagnosis by 24 months of age [2, 3, 2123]. A substantial portion of the remaining HRA infants who do not develop ASD begin to display subclinical autism-like behavioral traits (e.g., subthreshold ASD symptoms or subtle delays in language) indicative of the broader autism phenotype (BAP) by around 12 months of age [5]. Furthermore, before 12 months, HRA infants as a group exhibit subtle behavioral and neurological atypicalities in how they process and interact with the world [2430], potentially reflecting intermediate traits (‘endophenotypes’) of ASD [31, 32].

Subtle atypicalities related to behavioral and neurological processing of auditory and linguistic stimuli have been reported over the 1st year of life in HRA infants [26, 3336]. However, very few studies have examined responses to repeated auditory or linguistic stimuli, and only one has looked specifically at how the brain responds to auditory repetition. In that study, Guiraud et al. [26] used an ‘oddball paradigm’ to present 9-month-old infants with a repeated standard tone interspersed with infrequently presented deviant stimuli and then analyzed how a positive ERP component, the P150, changed in response to consecutive presentations of the standard. For low-risk control (LRC) infants, the amplitude of the P150 became progressively smaller after each presentation of the standard, reflecting neural habituation to the repetitive stimulus, while HRA infants in contrast failed to exhibit this neural habituation. That study, though, did not examine relationships between altered ERPs to auditory repetition and behavior, so the functional implications of this are currently unknown.

Even less is known about how HRA infants process linguistic repetition. Seery et al. [35] examined this to some degree using an oddball paradigm similar to Guiraud et al. [26] but with consonant-vowel speech stimuli rather than tones. Few group differences were evident between HRA and LRC infants in their P150s to the standard stimuli (although group differences in lateralization of a later ERP component were evident across condition types); however, this study only included standard stimuli that were immediately followed by a deviant (i.e., ‘pre-deviant’ standards) rather than examining consecutive presentations of the standard. It is possible then that HRA infants in that study did have altered responses to repetitions of the standard stimulus, similar to what has been reported for tones [26], but that this was not captured due to examining only pre-deviant trials.

In the current study, we explored the possibility that HRA infants have atypical ERPs to repetitive speech stimuli, potentially similar to what has been reported for tones [26], by focusing on infants’ P150s to consecutively presented standards in a sample expanded from Seery et al. [35]. In addition to examining whether ERPs to repetitive speech are altered in HRA infants, we sought to further understand the functional significance of any such atypicality by examining the relationships between ERPs to repetitive speech and infants’ later behavioral characteristics. Finally, to determine whether any such atypicality is specific to the repetitive nature of the stimulus or whether it would occur in response to speech in general, we also examined responses to the deviant stimulus, although our previous work suggested that HRA infants do not differ from low-risk infants in their P150s to deviant speech sounds.

Methods

The work presented here was part of a larger longitudinal investigation of infants at risk for ASD. Infants were recruited into the larger study between birth and 6 months and participated in a battery of behavioral, electrophysiological, and eye-tracking tasks during laboratory visits at several different ages (3, 6, 9, 12, 18, 24, and 36 months). The focus here is on portions of the electrophysiological data collected during the 9-month visit. All work was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Boston Children’s Hospital and Boston University, and upon enrollment, parents provided written consent for their infant to participate in the longitudinal study.

Participants

Two groups of infants (HRA and LRC) from monolingual, English-speaking households (English spoken ≥75% of the time) were enrolled. HRA infants each had an older sibling with ASD. Diagnoses in the older siblings were provided by expert community clinicians and were not due to known genetic disorders (e.g., fragile X syndrome), as determined by a detailed screening interview. LRC infants had at least one typically developing older sibling and no known first-degree relatives with ASD or other neurodevelopmental disorders, based on the screening interview. Infants were excluded if they had a gestational age less than 36 weeks, a genetic disorder known to be related to ASD, extensive perinatal/postnatal medical or neurological problems, or exposure to any language that uses the paradigm’s nonnative phonemic contrast (e.g., Hindi or Bengali; see the ‘Stimuli and procedure’ section for more details).

Usable ERP data were obtained from 85 9-month-old infants: 40 from HRA (mean age in days (SD) = 280.7(10.2); 21 male) and 45 from LRC (mean age in days (SD) = 281.3(10.8); 23 male). An additional 69 infants were tested but were not included in analyses due to the following: a) refusal to wear the ERP net, becoming too fussy after an initial visual ERP task, or not completing the task (7 HRA, 11 LRC); b) not providing enough artifact-free data due to excessive movement/fussiness or having excessively noisy data after editing (21 HRA, 26 LRC); c) experimenter/equipment error (1 HRA); d) English spoken in the house less than 75% of the time (2 HRA); or e) exposure to Hindi (1 HRA).

Because HRA infants who develop ASD differ in many ways from HRA infants who do not, and in order to better focus on the majority of infants who do not develop ASD, we excluded any infants with known ASD diagnoses from further analyses. Diagnoses were made using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) [37], administered at 24 and 36 months alongside expert clinical judgment at 36 months. The ADOS, a semi-structured play-based interaction designed to measure autism symptoms, provides a ‘severity score’ (1–10) to capture the presence of ASD symptoms, with higher scores indicating greater symptom severity and scores of 4 or higher being indicative of ASD. Infants were classified as having ASD if they scored at or above the ASD cutoff of 4 on their most recent 24- or 36-month ADOS and, for participants with 36-month outcome data available, received a clinical judgment rating of ‘ASD.’ Five HRA infants received diagnoses of ASD using these criteria (four from the ADOS at 36 months, one from the ADOS at 24 months) and were excluded from analyses. One LRC infant scored at the ASD cutoff on the ADOS at his 36-month visit; however, he scored below the threshold at 24 months and received a clinical judgment of ‘typically developing’ at 36 months, so he remained in the sample. Nine HRA infants and 10 LRC infants had not yet completed an ADOS at either 24 or 36 months and were allowed to remain in analyses.

Thirty-five HRA and 45 LRC infants were included in the final sample. Behavioral characteristics of the participants near the time of ERP collection (9 months) were obtained using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning [38], a developmental assessment, at 6 and 12 months. From the Mullen, we obtained standardized T-scores for four subscales: Fine Motor, Visual Reception, Expressive Language, and Receptive Language. In line with other work on this population, we found no difference between groups (using independent-samples t-tests) on subscale T-scores at 6 months (all p > 0.5) but found that HRA infants scored significantly lower than LRC infants on the Visual Reception and Receptive Language subscales at 12 months (all p < .05). Additional details about behavioral and demographic information for these groups are provided in Table 1. Note that 46 of these infants (23 HRA, 23 LRC) were included in Seery et al. [35].

Table 1 Characteristics of participants included in analyses

Behavioral characteristics of the infants were further assessed using the Mullen and ADOS at 18 months. Previous work has shown that at this age, traits of the broader autism phenotype (such as elevated ASD symptoms and language delay) are clearly evident, yet ASD symptoms are still not stable enough to allow for firm ASD diagnoses [5, 21]. As expected, at 18 months, HRA infants performed significantly lower than LRC infants on the Visual Reception (p = .035), Receptive Language (p < .001), and Expressive Language (p = .019) subscales of the Mullen and had significantly higher ADOS severity scores than LRC infants (p = .002). See Table 1 for more details as well as the number of infants who provided Mullen and ADOS scores at each age.

Stimuli and procedure

A stream of consonant-vowel stimuli was presented to infants using a double oddball paradigm. A standard stimulus (voiced, unaspirated, retroflex stop; /ɖa/) was presented 80% of the time, and the primary deviant stimulus (voiceless, aspirated retroflex palatal stop; /ta/) was randomly interspersed 10% of the time. A second, nonnative language deviant (voiced, unaspirated dental stop; /da/) was presented the remaining 10% of the time. English does not differentiate between the voiced retroflex and dental stops, so adult monolingual English speakers perceive both the standard and the nonnative deviant simply as /da/. In contrast, these sounds can be easily distinguished by adult speakers of languages that use these sounds contrastively (e.g., Bengali or Hindi) and by very young infants being exposed to any language.

A maximum of 600 stimuli, each 300 ms in duration, were presented at 80 db over two bilateral speakers using an interstimulus interval with offset-to-onset times varying between 1,100 and 1,400 ms. Throughout the procedure, infants were seated on a parent’s lap in a sound-attenuated, dimly lit room and wore either a 64-channel Geodesic Sensor Net or 128-channel HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net (Electrical Geodesics Inc., Eugene, OR; change in net type was due to a system upgrade partway through the longitudinal project) from which we recorded continuous electroencephalogram (EEG). To maintain infants’ interest and increase toleration of the electrode net, an experimenter was present and provided the infant with opportunities for quiet toy play, bubble blowing, feeding, or other similar activities. On average, the procedure took approximately 15 min. More detailed information about stimulus creation and procedure has been provided previously in Seery et al. [35].

Analysis of electrophysiological data

Continuous EEG was referenced online to vertex (Cz), amplified with a 0.1-to-100-Hz band-pass filter using a NetAmp200 or NetAmp300 amplifier (due to a system upgrade partway through the longitudinal project), and digitized at 250H using NetStation software (Electrical Geodesics Inc.). EEG was segmented into 800-ms epochs starting 100 ms before stimulus onset, digitally filtered using a 30-Hz low-pass elliptical filter and baseline-corrected using mean voltage during the 100-ms pre-stimulus baseline period.

Segments were visually examined for artifacts, and individual channels were marked as bad if contaminated by artifacts such as body movement, eye movement, eye-blinks, or off-scale activity (±200 μV). If more than 15% of the channels in a given segment were marked as bad, that entire segment was excluded from analyses. Participants with fewer than ten acceptable segments in any stimulus category (see below) were excluded from all analyses. For the remaining participants, the bad channels of accepted segments were replaced using spherical spline interpolation, then average waveforms for each condition were calculated and re-referenced to the average reference.

For the current study, we were specifically interested in how infants’ brains respond to repetitions of the standard stimulus. Note that infants’ ability to detect nonnative phonemic contrasts changes drastically over the 1st year of life, thus impacting their ERPs to the nonnative deviant stimulus [39, 40]. Examination of this developmental change is outside the scope of the current project, so responses to the nonnative deviant were not examined here (although the reader is directed to Seery et al. [35]). Instead, analyses were restricted to the native deviant (hereafter referred to simply as the deviant) as well as ‘runs’ of consecutively presented instances of the standard stimulus. Following Guiraud et al. [26], we segmented the continuous EEG into four categories of stimuli. The first category consisted of the deviant stimulus (/ta/), and the remaining three categories were constructed from the runs of repeated standards that began immediately after this deviant (e.g., /da/ /ta/ /da/ /da/ /da/). All standards that immediately followed a native deviant were included in the ‘first standard’ category (S1; /ta/ / da /). The ‘second standard’ (S2) category included all standards that immediately followed an S1 (i.e., the two stimuli presented just before the S2 were the deviant and then an S1; /ta/ /da/ / da /). Finally, the ‘third standard’ (S3) category included all standards that immediately followed an S2 (/ta/ /da/ /da/ / da /).

It should be noted that since the paradigm was designed such that deviant stimuli were randomly interspersed throughout the procedure, there were fewer instances of S2 stimuli than S1 and fewer S3 than S2 (due to the deviant stimuli ‘interrupting’ the runs of consecutive standard stimuli). The number of usable trials per condition directly contributes to the signal-to-noise ratio of ERPs, and the signal-to-noise ratio can have an impact on both the maximum amplitude and latency of ERPs [41]. Therefore, we analyzed average amplitude of the waveform rather than maximum amplitude or latency to the peak, since average amplitude is less impacted by differences in number of trials [41]. The number of usable trials did not differ across groups for any of the four conditions (all p > .05; see Table 1).

Based on previous work, we focused analyses on an early positive component, the P150, which is sensitive to stimulus deviance and is maximal over frontal and central electrodes [26, 35, 42]. Specifically, we analyzed the average amplitude of the waveform from 150–300-ms post stimulus onset over four regions of interest (ROIs) computed from frontal and central electrodes from the left and right hemisphere (see Figure 1 for details).

Figure 1
figure 1

Regions of interest. Electrode groupings used for the 64-channel Geodesic Sensor Net (on left) and the 128-channel HydroCel Sensor Net (on right). The frontal regions of interest (ROIs) consisted of electrodes F1, F3, F7, and AF3 on the left and F2, F4, F8, and AF4 on the right. Central regions of interest consisted of FC1, FC5, C3, and C5 on the left and FC2, FC6, C4, and C6 on the right.

Results

Following Guiraud et al. [26], and in line with the focus of this study, we focused primarily on responses to the repetition of the standard stimulus. Specifically, we examined whether infants differ in how they respond to the first, second, and third consecutive repetitions of this stimuli and whether HRA and LRC infants differ from each other. We next examined the relationships between these responses at 9 months and later behavior at 18 months. Finally, in order to understand whether any potential atypicalities are related specifically to the repetition of the stimulus or whether they are more generally related to speech processing, we examined infants’ responses to the deviant stimulus.

Mixed-model ANOVAs were used, with Greenhouse-Geisser corrections applied as needed, and significant effects were examined further using reduced ANOVAs, independent-sample t-tests, or paired-sample t-tests, with Bonferroni corrections applied when appropriate. An alpha level of .05 was used throughout.Waveform graphs for the three standard stimuli as well as for the deviant are given in Figure 2.

Figure 2
figure 2

Grand-averaged waveforms for LRC and HRA infants. Grand-averaged waveform graphs over each ROI for (a) LRC and (b) HRA groups for the first (S1), second (S2), and third (S3) standards as well as the deviant (Dev) condition.

How do infants respond to repeated speech?

To examine how infants respond to repetitions of the standard, we performed separate mixed-model ANOVAs for frontal and central ROIs using condition (first standard, second standard, third standard) and hemisphere (left, right) as repeated factors and group (HRA, LRC) as a between-subjects factor with the average amplitude of the P150 as the dependent variable.

Over frontal electrodes, this three-way ANOVA revealed no significant effects of condition but did reveal a main effect of group (F(1,78) = 4.57, p = .036; partial eta squared = .055) such that average amplitudes were larger (more positive) for HRA infants (mean = 2.90 μV, SD = 2.54) than LRC infants (mean = 1.76 μV, SD = 2.24; see Figure 3a). There were no significant effects or interactions over central electrodes (all p > .20).

Figure 3
figure 3

Amplitude of the P150 to standard and deviant stimuli. Amplitude of P150 for HRA (red) and LRC (blue) infants. (a) Response to the three standard conditions (S1 = first standard, S2 = second standard, S3 = third standard); (b) response to the deviant.

What is the relationship between infants’ ERPs to repeated speech and later behavior?

Next, we investigated the functional implications of elevated ERPs to the standards in HRA infants by exploring the relationship with later behavioral traits at 18 months. As there were no differences in response to the first, second, or third repetitions of the standard in either group or across hemisphere, we averaged the ERPs across these three conditions and across hemispheres. As noted earlier, at 18 months HRA infants had lower Visual Reception, Expressive Language, and Receptive Language T-scores as well as higher ADOS severity scores than LRC infants (see Table 1).

We were interested in and anticipated potentially different relationships between ERPs and behavior for LRC versus HRA infants, so we analyzed each group separately, computing bivariate Pearson correlations between the average response to the standards at 9 months and Mullen verbal (Expressive Language, Receptive Language) and nonverbal ability (Visual Reception, Fine Motor) at 18 months. For the HRA infants, we also examined the relationship with autism symptoms by computing nonparametric Spearman correlations between ERPs and ADOS severity scores at 18 months (LRC infants did not have enough variance in severity scores to allow for meaningful correlations).

For LRC infants, there were no significant correlations between ERPs and any of the behavioral variables. In contrast, for HRA infants, there were positive correlations between amplitude of the P150 to the standards and Expressive Language score over both frontal (Pearson’s r = .360, p = .051) and central ROIs (Pearson’s r = .442, p = .015). A summary of correlation coefficients is given in Table 2, and scatter plots for the relationships between amplitude of the P150 and Expressive Language are given in Figure 4.

Table 2 Correlation coefficients between amplitude of P150 to the standards at 9 months by ROI and behavioral variables at 18 months a
Figure 4
figure 4

Correlations between P150 to the standards at 9 months and Expressive Language at 18 months. Scatterplots of the relationships between amplitude of the P150 to the standards at 9 months and Expressive Language T-scores at 18 months for HRA and LRC infants over frontal and central ROIs. Correlation coefficients are given in Table 2. Note that correlations remain significant when excluding the two HRA participants with lowest language scores.*p ≤ .05

How do infants respond to deviant speech?

We next examined responses to the deviant by computing mixed-model ANOVAs for frontal and central ROIs using hemisphere (left, right) as a repeated factor and group (HRA, LRC) as a between-subjects factor with average amplitude of the P150 to the deviant as the dependent variable.

Over frontal electrodes, this revealed no significant main effects or interactions. Over central electrodes, the ANOVA revealed a main effect of hemisphere (F(1,83) = 7.03, p = .010; partial eta squared = .083) such that amplitude was larger over the left hemisphere (mean = 4.64 μV, SD = 3.61) than right (mean = 3.27 μV, SD = 3.63), but no effect of group, suggesting that the response to the deviant did not differ between HRA and LRC infants (Figure 3b).

Finally, we computed an omnibus ANOVA including standards and deviants with condition (first standard, second standard, third standard, deviant) and hemisphere (left, right) as repeated factors and group (HRA, LRC) as a between-subjects factor.

Over frontal electrodes, this ANOVA revealed a main effect of condition (F(3,234) = 4.50, p = .005; partial eta squared = .054) modulated by a condition by hemisphere interaction (F(3,234) = 3.14, p = .027, partial eta squared = .039). The condition by hemisphere interaction was driven by the fact that within the left hemisphere, the deviant was significantly larger than the first (p < .001) and third standards (p = .013), while in the right hemisphere there were no differences between conditions (all p > .1). Furthermore, the first standard was larger over the right than the left hemisphere (p = .014). There were no other significant effects or interactions.

Over the central ROI, this ANOVA revealed only a main effect of condition (F(3,234) = 5.62, p = .001; partial eta squared = .067), such that response to the deviant was larger than to the first (p = .001) and third standards (p = .013), although it did not differ from the second standard (p = .136). Response to the three standards did not differ from each other (all p > .2).

Discussion

In this study, we examined how unaffected 9-month-old infants at high risk for ASD respond to repeated speech sounds. Analyses focused on the average amplitude of a positive ERP component, the P150, over frontal and central electrodes in response to three consecutive repetitions of a standard consonant-vowel stimulus. Overall, we found that for frontal electrodes, the amplitude of the P150 to repetitions of the standard was larger for HRA infants than for LRC infants. Furthermore, for HRA infants only, amplitude of ERPs to the standards was positively related to later language ability.

First, consider the LRC infants, whose P150s to the standard did not change in amplitude in response to successive presentations of the standard. In previous work with infants of the same age but using nonlinguistic stimuli, Guiraud et al. [26] reported that the P150 dampened with each consecutive presentation of a repetitive tone, suggesting neural habituation. The difference in these findings may be due in part to the linguistic nature of our stimuli. It is known that typically developing infants prefer to listen to speech over nonspeech sounds and have larger P150s in response to speech sounds than nonspeech sounds [43, 44]. Furthermore, although localization of the neural sources of infant ERP components is challenging, previous work suggests that the infant auditory P150 has generators in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) as well as the left and right auditory temporal cortices [42]. When the P150 is elicited by speech sounds, there is evidence that the ACC, which is involved in modulation of attention, is activated before the auditory cortex [42]. The opposite activation pattern (activation of the auditory cortex followed by the ACC) has been found for nonspeech sounds [45], suggesting that attention may play a vital role in the generation of the P150 when infants encounter linguistic sounds. It is possible as well that evidence for neural habituation would be found if we had examined more than three consecutive presentations of the standard (for example, including out to the fourth or fifth standard); however, this was not possible with our data due to a relatively small number of fourth and fifth standard trials. Despite the lack of dampening of the P150 to the repeated syllable, it was clear that infants in our sample were sensitive to the repeated nature of the stimulus, as they showed a strong mismatch response to the deviant as evidenced by larger amplitude to the deviant than the first and third standards.

Next, consider the HRA infants, who, like the LRC infants, had consistent amplitude of the P150 in response to the three consecutive presentations of the standard, although this amplitude was larger than that of LRC infants. These atypically large responses to the standard stimuli provide evidence that processing of repetitive speech sounds by unaffected HRA infants is altered relative to the typical population. This finding builds upon previous work surrounding nonlinguistic auditory repetition processing in infant and adult participants with a family history of ASD [12, 26] by providing evidence that this extends to the linguistic domain during infancy. Importantly, atypically large ERPs to the repetitive speech sounds in HRA infants were associated with better development, so atypically elevated responses in our sample appear to be indicative of beneficial rather than impaired processing, particularly as this relates to language acquisition.

There are a few potential explanations for why HRA infants in our study had atypically elevated responses to the repeated speech sounds and why this was associated with better language development. One possibility is that HRA infants experience atypical attention modulation and integration that affects their ERPs to the stimuli during this passive task. There is evidence that HRA infants and older first-degree relatives of individuals with ASD have atypical attention styles [29, 4648], lending support to this hypothesis. In this scenario, elevated responses to the standards could arise from HRA infants actively attending or listening to the ‘background’ repetitive speech stimuli, in contrast to the LRC infants who may be better at simultaneously attending to multiple features in their environment (e.g., bubbles, toys, food, or other sounds that are present during the task in addition to the background syllables). Previous work suggests that there is variation among infants in their relative preference for listening to speech over other nonlinguistic auditory stimuli and that, as a group, HRA infants show dampened preference for speech over nonspeech sounds relative to LRC infants [36]. However, those HRA infants who do prefer to listen to speech have better language ability and later show fewer ASD symptoms [36]. Our findings, then, may be driven by the subset of HRA infants who prefer to attend to speech over nonspeech sounds as these infants may have elevated P150s (due to attending to the speech stimuli) as well as higher later-language ability. If this hypothesis is true, then we would not necessarily expect to find the same functional significance for atypical responses to repetitive non linguistic stimuli. Specifically, if response is governed by attention to the stimuli, then previously reported dampened habituation and/or atypically large responses to repeated nonlinguistic sounds in high-risk relatives may be associated with language or sensory integration difficulties. In line with this idea, recent work suggests that children with ASD with auditory hypersensitivity have atypically large neural responses to repeated tones, while children without auditory hypersensitivity do not [49]. Much more work is required in order to understand whether this is the case in unaffected family members as well.

Another potential explanation for the elevated amplitudes to repeated speech sounds in HRA infants is that this arises not from differences in attention but from altered processing of speech more generally. For example, it may be that HRA infants experience difficulties with neural auditory or linguistic processing and that the HRA infants who develop better language also allocate more, or potentially alternative, cognitive or neural resources to processing these stimuli, resulting in larger-amplitude ERPs to the standard. Recent work suggests that brain areas associated with language processing, including the temporal cortex, have atypicalities from early in life in individuals with ASD [50], which could result in impaired auditory and linguistic processing in ASD; however, this has not yet been studied during the 1st year of life in HRA infants.

Future work is needed to elucidate the nature of our findings; however, these data at least provide support for the idea that atypical neural processing of auditory repetition may be an endophenotype of ASD. Notably, HRA infants did not differ from LRC infants in their responses to the deviant, suggesting that this endophenotype may be specific to the processing of repetitive stimuli. Although we did not find a significant group-by-condition interaction between how HRA and LRC infants processed standard and deviant stimuli, providing some hesitation to this interpretation, previous work also suggests no differences between HRA and LRC infants in P150s to deviant speech stimuli [35]. More work should continue to explore this issue.

Our work has some limitations that should be considered when interpreting the findings. First, we examined only responses to linguistic stimuli, so it is unclear whether our findings are specific to linguistic stimuli or whether they hold also for nonlinguistic auditory stimuli. As discussed earlier, future work should directly address this empirical question. Second, we did not examine infants who develop ASD, so it is unclear whether similar responses would also be found in HRA infants who do ultimately receive diagnoses of ASD. Comparison of infants who develop ASD against HRA infants who do not may additionally help to understand previously reported differences in neural responses to auditory/linguistic stimuli between older children with ASD and their first-degree relatives. It should also be noted that it is possible that some HRA infants from our sample may ultimately develop ASD as not all infants had reached an age where diagnosis is possible.

Conclusions

In sum, we found evidence that atypically large ERPs to repeated speech sounds are present in unaffected 9-month-old HRA infants and may be an endophenotype of ASD. Large responses were associated with better developmental outcome, suggesting that this response pattern, although atypical, is not indicative of disordered processing. Although more work is needed in order to understand these findings, this adds to a growing body of literature suggesting that HRA infants differ from low-risk infants in how they process and interact with their auditory world.

Abbreviations

ACC:

anterior cingulate cortex

ADOS:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

ASD:

autism spectrum disorder

BAP:

broader autism phenotype

EEG:

electroencephalogram

ERP:

event-related potential

HRA:

high-risk for autism

LRC:

low-risk control

ROI:

region of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5. 2013, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 5

    Google Scholar 

  2. Ozonoff S, Iosif AM, Baguio F, Cook IC, Hill MM, Hutman T, Rogers SJ, Rozga A, Sangha S, Sigman M: A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010, 49: 256-266.e2.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Rogers T, Roberts W, Brian J, Szatmari P: Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005, 23: 143-152. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Elsabbagh M, Johnson MH: Getting answers from babies about autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010, 14: 81-87. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Belding A, Hill M, Hill A, Hutman T, Johnson S, Miller M, Rogers SJ, Schwichtenberg AJ, Steinfeld M, Iosif A-M: The broader autism phenotype in infancy: when does it emerge?. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014, 53: 398-407.e2. 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.020.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Messinger D, Young GS, Ozonoff S, Dobkins K, Carter A, Zwaigenbaum L, Landa RJ, Charman T, Stone WL, Constantino JN, Hutman T, Carver LJ, Bryson S, Iverson JM, Strauss MS, Rogers SJ, Sigman M: Beyond autism: a baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013, 52: 300.e1-308.e1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Gamliel I, Yirmiya N, Jaffe DH, Manor O, Sigman M: Developmental trajectories in siblings of children with autism: cognition and language from 4 months to 7 years. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009, 39: 1131-1144. 10.1007/s10803-009-0727-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Whitehouse AJO, Bishop DVM: Do children with autism “switch off” to speech sounds? An investigation using event-related potentials. Dev Sci. 2008, 11: 516-524. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00697.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Lepisto T, Kujala T, Vanhala R, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Naatanen R: The discrimination of and orienting to speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism. Brain Res. 2005, 1066: 147-157. 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Čeponienē R, Lepistö T, Shestakova A, Vanhala R, Alku P, Näätänen R, Yaguchi K: Speech–sound-selective auditory impairment in children with autism: they can perceive but do not attend. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003, 100: 5567-5572. 10.1073/pnas.0835631100.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Donkers FCL, Schipul SE, Baranek GT, Cleary KM, Willoughby MT, Evans AM, Bulluck JC, Lovmo JE, Belger A: Attenuated auditory event-related potentials and associations with atypical sensory response patterns in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013

    Google Scholar 

  12. Jansson-Verkasalo E, Kujala T, Jussila K, Mattila ML, Moilanen I, Näätänen R, Suominen K, Korpilahti P: Similarities in the phenotype of the auditory neural substrate in children with Asperger syndrome and their parents. Eur J Neurosci. 2005, 22: 986-990. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04216.x.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Buchwald J, Erwin R, Van Lancker D, Guthrie D, Schwafel J, Tanguay P: Midlatency auditory evoked responses: PI abnormalities in adult autistic subjects. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1992, 84: 164-171. 10.1016/0168-5597(92)90021-3.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ceponiene R, Kielinen M, Suominen K, Jäntti V, Linna SL, Moilanen I, Näätänen R: Deficient auditory processing in children with Asperger Syndrome, as indexed by event-related potentials. Neurosci Lett. 2003, 338: 197-200. 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)01405-2.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Martineau J, Roux S, Garreau B, Adrien J, Lelord G: Unimodal and crossmodal reactivity in autism: presence of auditory evoked responses and effect of the repetition of auditory stimuli. Biol Psychiatry. 1992, 31: 1190-1203. 10.1016/0006-3223(92)90338-Z.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Stroganova T, Kozunov V, Posikera I, Galuta I, Gratchev V, Orekhova E: Abnormal pre-attentive arousal in young children with autism spectrum disorder contributes to their atypical auditory behavior: an ERP study. PLoS One. 2013, 8: 1-23.e69100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Buard I, Rogers SJ, Hepburn S, Kronberg E, Rojas DC: Altered oscillation patterns and connectivity during picture naming in autism. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013, 7: 1-11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. McFadden K, Hepburn S, Winterrowd E, Schmidt G, Rojas D: Abnormalities in gamma-band responses to language stimuli in first-degree relatives of children with autism spectrum disorder: an MEG study. BMC Psychiatry. 2012, 12: 213-10.1186/1471-244X-12-213.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Rojas DC, Teale PD, Maharajh K, Kronberg E, Youngpeter K, Wilson LB, Wallace A, Hepburn S: Transient and steady-state auditory gamma-band responses in first-degree relatives of people with autism spectrum disorder. Mol Autism. 2011, 2: 11-10.1186/2040-2392-2-11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, Messinger D, Yirmiya N, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Carver LJ, Constantino JN, Dobkins K, Hutman T, Iverson JM, Landa R, Rogers SJ, Sigman M, Stone WL: Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a baby siblings research consortium study. Pediatrics. 2011, 128: e488-e495.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Lord C, Rogers S, Carter A, Carver L, Chawarska K, Constantino J, Dawson G, Dobkins K, Fein D, Iverson J, Klin A, Landa R, Messinger D, Ozonoff S, Sigman M, Stone W, Tager-Flusberg H, Yirmiya N: Clinical assessment and management of toddlers with suspected autism spectrum disorder: insights from studies of high-risk infants. Pediatrics. 2009, 123: 1383-1391. 10.1542/peds.2008-1606.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Mitchell S, Brian J, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Szatmari P, Smith I, Bryson S: Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006, 27 (2 Suppl): S69-S78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ozonoff S, Macari S, Young G, Goldring S, Thompson M, Rogers S: Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism. 2008, 12: 457-472. 10.1177/1362361308096402.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. De Klerk CCJM, Gliga T, Charman T, Johnson MH: Face engagement during infancy predicts later face recognition ability in younger siblings of children with autism. Dev Sci. 2014, 17: 596-611. 10.1111/desc.12141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Georgiades S, Szatmari P, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Brian J, Roberts W, Smith I, Vaillancourt T, Roncadin C, Garon N: A prospective study of autistic-like traits in unaffected siblings of probands with autism spectrum disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2013, 70: 42-48.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Guiraud JA, Kushnerenko E, Tomalski P, Davies K, Ribeiro H, Johnson MH: Differential habituation to repeated sounds in infants at high risk for autism. Neuroreport. 2011, 22: 845-849.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Guiraud JA, Tomalski P, Kushnerenko E, Ribeiro H, Davies K, Charman T, Elsabbagh M, Johnson MH: Atypical audiovisual speech integration in infants at risk for autism. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e36428-10.1371/journal.pone.0036428.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Drouker D, Curtin S, Vouloumanos A: Linking infant-directed-speech and face preferences to language outcomes in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2013, 56: 567-576. 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0266).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Elsabbagh M, Volein A, Holmboe K, Tucker L, Csibra G, Baron-cohen S, Bolton P, Charman T, Baird G, Johnson MH, Mark H: Visual orienting in the early broader autism phenotype: disengagement and facilitation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009, 50: 637-642. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02051.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. McCleery JP, Akshoomoff N, Dobkins KR, Carver LJ: Atypical face versus object processing and hemispheric asymmetries in 10-month-old infants at risk for autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2009, 66: 950-957. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.031.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Gottesman I, Gould T: The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003, 160: 636-645. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Viding E, Blakemore SJ: Endophenotype approach to developmental psychopathology: implications for autism research. Behav Genet. 2007, 37: 51-60. 10.1007/s10519-006-9105-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Lloyd-Fox S, Blasi A, Elwell CE, Charman T, Murphy D, Johnson MH: Reduced neural sensitivity to social stimuli in infants at risk for autism. Proc R Soc B. 2013, 280: 20123026-10.1098/rspb.2012.3026.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Ference J, Curtin S: Attention to lexical stress and early vocabulary growth in 5-month-olds at risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013, 116: 891-903. 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Seery A, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson C: Atypical lateralization of ERP response to native and non-native speech in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2013, 5: 10-24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Curtin S, Vouloumanos A: Speech preference is associated with autistic-like behavior in 18-month-olds at risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013, 43: 2114-2120. 10.1007/s10803-013-1759-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore P, Pickles A, Rutter M: The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule––Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000, 30: 205-223. 10.1023/A:1005592401947.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Mullen E: Mullen Scales of Early Learning. 1995, Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc

    Google Scholar 

  39. Werker JF, Tees R: Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behav Dev. 1984, 7: 49-63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Rivera-Gaxiola M, Silva-Pereyra J, Kuhl PK: Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7-and 11-month-old American infants. Dev Sci. 2005, 8: 162-172. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00403.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Luck S: An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. 2005, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  42. Ortiz-Mantilla S, Hämäläinen JA, Benasich AA: Time course of ERP generators to syllables in infants: a source localization study using age-appropriate brain templates. Neuroimage. 2012, 59: 3275-3287. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.048.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Dehaene-Lambertz G: Cerebral specialization for speech and non-speech stimuli in infants. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000, 12: 449-460. 10.1162/089892900562264.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Vouloumanos A, Werker JF: Listening to language at birth: evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Dev Sci. 2007, 10: 159-164. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00549.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Hämäläinen JA, Ortiz-Mantilla S, Benasich AA: Source localization of event-related potentials to pitch change mapped onto age-appropriate MRIs at 6 months of age. Neuroimage. 2011, 54: 1910-1918. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Belmonte M, Gomot M, Baron-Cohen S: Visual attention in autism families: “unaffected” sibs share atypical frontal activation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010, 51: 2010-

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Happé F, Briskman J, Frith U: Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: weak “central coherence” in parents and siblings of children with autism: I. Experimental tests. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001, 42: 299-307. 10.1111/1469-7610.00723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Briskman J, Happé F, Frith U: Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: weak “central coherence” in parents and siblings of children with autism: II. Real-life skills and preferences. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001, 42: 309-316. 10.1111/1469-7610.00724.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Matsuzaki J, Kagitani-Shimono K, Sugata H, Hirata M, Hanaie R, Nagatani F, Tachibana M, Tominaga K, Mohri I, Taniike M: Progressively increased M50 responses to repeated sounds in autism spectrum disorder with auditory hypersensitivity: a magnetoencephalographic study. PLoS One. 2014, 9: e102599-10.1371/journal.pone.0102599.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Eyler LT, Pierce K, Courchesne E: A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain. 2012, 135: 949-960. 10.1093/brain/awr364.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by funding from the NIDCD (R21 DC 08637, to HTF; R01 DC 10290, to HTF and CN), the Simons Foundation (137186, to CN), the Autism Speaks Pilot Grants Program (to HTF), and the Autism Speaks Weatherstone Fellowship Program (to AS). We would like to thank the participants and families for their time and dedication to the project. Furthermore, thank you to the large community of staff and students who have been involved with the Infant Sibling Project over the years and who have contributed to data collection, data processing, and helpful discussions regarding data analysis. Finally, thank you to all of the interns who have helped with the data processing, in particular for this project Rakhi Desai, Hana Kahn, and Alexandra Yellin.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne Seery.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

AS, HTF, and CN conceived of the study and its design. AS processed and analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to interpretation of the data, intellectual content for the manuscript, and critical revisions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Seery, A., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Nelson, C.A. Event-related potentials to repeated speech in 9-month-old infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodevelop Disord 6, 43 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-6-43

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-6-43

Keywords