Skip to main content

Imaging episodic memory during development and childhood epilepsy

Abstract

Epilepsy affects 2.2 million adults in the USA, with 1 in 26 people developing epilepsy at some point in their lives. Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is the most common form of focal epilepsy as medial structures, and the hippocampus in particular, are prone to generating seizures. Selective anterior temporal resection (which removes the hippocampus) is the most effective intractable TLE treatment, but given the critical role of the mesial temporal lobe in memory functioning, resection can have negative effects on this crucial cognitive skill. To minimize the adverse impact of temporal lobe surgery on memory functioning, reliable pre-surgical guides are needed. Clinical functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) provides reliable, noninvasive guidance of language functioning and plays a growing role in the pre-surgical evaluation for epilepsy patients; however, localization of memory function in children with epilepsy using fMRI has not been established. Aside from the lack of neuroimaging memory studies in children with TLE, studies of typical development are limited. This review will focus on the functional anatomy of memory systems throughout development, with a focus on TLE. TLE provides the ideal model from which to understand memory function and the limits of plasticity and compensation/reorganization throughout development.

Introduction

Memory is the process by which information is encoded, stored, and retrieved [1]. Memory can be divided into explicit (declarative) and implicit (nondeclarative) domains based on the manner information is stored and recalled [2]. Explicit memory encompasses factual knowledge (semantic memory) and memory for events and personal experiences (episodic memory) and involves at least four processes, including encoding, consolidation, storage, and retrieval. This review will focus on episodic memory, which is thought to develop through childhood. Both lesion and neuroimaging studies delineate the neural systems involved in episodic memory. Ghetti and Bunge [3] state that the development of episodic memory occurs through development of a brain network including the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and posterior parietal cortex.

Although episodic memory involves a distributed network, the medial temporal lobe (MTL), and specifically the hippocampus, has a particular role in episodic memory encoding and retrieval [4]. Many of the foundational lesion-based studies of memory were based on patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), and TLE provides a model to understand memory development. Thus, TLE will be a focus of the current review.

Memory and epilepsy

Adults

The MTL has a specific role in memory encoding and retrieval [4], yet this region is also prone to generating seizures, making TLE a common form of focal epilepsy [5]. Memory impairments are a common comorbidity in epilepsy [6,7,8,9]. Adults with TLE demonstrate material-specific memory deficits ipsilateral to seizure foci [6,7,8,9]—with left TLE associated with lower verbal memory on neuropsychological assessment and right TLE associated with lower visual memory [8, 9]. Material specificity describes each hippocampus’ processing of specific stimulus types (verbal—left; visual—right) and has been a hallmark concept in the neuropsychology of TLE. However, it is important to note that this theory may be oversimplified given the complexity of memory [10]. It has been posited that TLE patients with mesial temporal sclerosis (MTS) may show more material-specific deficits than those with more lateral TLE. Castro et al. [11] addressed this specific question and found that lateralization of verbal and nonverbal memory deficits was highly specific for left and right MTS (left MTS 82.2%, right MTS 92%) but infrequent (left MTS 11/43 or 25.6%, right 11/42 or 26.2%). Thus, there is evidence that material-specific memory deficits occur in TLE, particularly with MTS, but they do not occur ubiquitously.

Epilepsy surgery studies, however, provide further support for material specificity. The most effective intractable TLE treatment is selective anterior temporal resection [12, 13]; however, this can have negative effects on memory given the role of the MTL in memory functioning [14,15,16,17,18]. Postoperative decline in memory skills has been documented which correlates with the side of focus/surgical intervention [14,15,16, 19, 20], providing additional support for the material specificity hypothesis. Preoperative verbal memory performance (via neuropsychological assessment) predicts postoperative memory outcome for adults after left anterior temporal resections [6, 21].

Children

Seventy percent of children and adolescents with epilepsy describe problems with learning and memory [22], and both parents and patients indicate that the cognitive effects of epilepsy are a primary concern [23]. For children, characterizing and understanding these impairments is important because learning and memory are essential skills for success in academic and everyday functioning [24], including adaptive functioning, quality of life, and eventual employment. Children with epilepsy exhibit deficits in learning and memory [25, 26]; however, the severity and pattern of impairment is not clear, with findings ranging from global memory disruption [27,28,29] to no deficits [30, 31]. For pediatric TLE, although material-specific memory deficits have also been found [29, 32], the majority of studies do not show the adult pattern of pre-surgical lateralizing memory impairments [8, 33, 34]. One study found that adolescents with TLE (along with adults), but not children, show laterality differences in memory performance [8]. This provides evidence that the lateralizing profile may be more evident with age. Instead of this lateralizing profile, there is evidence that children with TLE have memory functioning similar to typically developing (TD) controls [33] and that patients with mesial TLE have worse memory functioning than patients with lateral TLE [34]. Furthermore, regardless of side of focus, verbal memory may be most affected in pediatric TLE [35] and in pediatric epilepsy in general [36].

In surgical populations, overall, children with epilepsy do not show material-specific memory impairments postoperatively. A review reported that postoperative memory decline was noted in four out of 13 studies, while six out of 13 documented no change, and three out of 13 showed an improvement in memory functioning [37]. These results suggest children, unlike adults, are less reliant on the dominant MTL for memory functioning. However, Law et al. [38] compared temporal lobe resections with and without inclusion of the mesial structures (hippocampus and amygdala). They found that patients (age 5–19) with left mesial temporal lobe resections had declines in verbal memory, and thus, the authors suggest that previous studies did not analyze the surgical details when discussing postoperative memory decline. Further, children in this group were most at risk for verbal memory decline when they had (1) typical language lateralization (i.e., left dominant) and (2) good preoperative verbal memory (> 85 Standard Score). In sum, patients with left mesial temporal resections may have postoperative declines in verbal memory (but not to the extent observed in adults), while children with extra-mesial resections do not.

Overall, important differences exist between children and adults in epilepsy and the effects of temporal resection on memory networks. Children with TLE may not show the adult pattern of pre- or post-surgical lateralizing memory impairments, but there is some recent evidence that those with mesial temporal resections might. Neuroimaging may help to answer many of these questions regarding developmental differences in hippocampal functioning.

Neuroimaging (fMRI) and memory

Task-based fMRI

Typical development

A complete review of memory fMRI functioning for adult TD populations is beyond the scope of the current review (for review, see Rugg and Vilberg, [39]). Rugg and Vilberg [39] review two of the main types of memory fMRI paradigms employed that dissociate recollection and familiarity. One uses the “Remember/Know” procedure, where participants report whether recognition of an item is accompanied (Remember) or unaccompanied (Know) by retrieval of contextual details during studying. The other requires a judgment about contextual features during studying (a “source memory” judgment, e.g., color of font). In sum, recollection-sensitive fMRI effects have been found in the hippocampus, parahippocampal, retrosplenial/posterior cingulate, lateral parietal cortices, and mPFC. There is evidence from adult TD studies that activity in the hippocampus increases when retrieval is associated with conscious recollection of a learning episode, and not when items are recognized only by familiarity or unrecognized [40] (see Fig. 1). Further, fMRI provides evidence for material specificity, such that each hippocampus is specialized to process specific types of information in TD adults; with verbal encoding resulting in left lateralization [41, 42] and visual encoding in right [41] (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Results from hippocampal region of interest, comparing correct R and correct K response amplitudes (a). Event-related responses within the left hippocampal region; response amplitude for correct R trials was greater than that for correct K trials (b). Adapted with permission from Eldridge et al. [40]

Fig. 2
figure 2

Activation in MTL for two participants. Pattern encoding yielded right, word yielded left, and face and scene encoding yielded bilateral activation. Adapted with permission from Golby et al. [96]

Studies of memory functioning in TD children using fMRI are sparse (for review, see Ghetti and Bunge, [3]; see Table 1). The majority have focused on visual memory, memory for detail/complexity, or longitudinal hippocampal activation. Regarding visual memory, during encoding of visual scenes in children (age 11–19), one study found that MTL response decreased with age, while activation in the left dorsolateral PFC increased with age [14]. Similarly, Ofen et al. [43] studied adults and children (age 8–24) during encoding of visual scenes and found greater activation in subsequently remembered scenes than in forgotten scenes in the MTL and PFC areas and that the activation within the PFC increased with age. In a study of spatial memory, adults (age 18–25) demonstrated activation in the head and body of the hippocampus, but children (age 8–9 and 10–11) did not [44]. Differences between the age groups were also found in the posterior parietal cortex, anterior prefrontal cortex, and insula. These age-related differences in hippocampal activation are similar even in studies of verbal memory. A study of verbal memory in TD children (age 7–19) showed left hippocampal and basal ganglia activation for verbal memory in children, which declined as age increased [16].

Several studies have found differences in recollection for detail or complexity throughout development. Ghetti et al. [45] used an incidental encoding task to study groups of children across the age span (8-year-olds, 10–11-year-olds, 14-year-olds, and young adults) and found functional changes across development such that the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus became increasingly more specialized for detailed recollection. Chai et al. [46] examined indoor and outdoor scenes of varying complexity in children and young adults (age 8–24) and found that recognition memory improved with age for high-, but not low-complexity scenes [46]. The authors concluded that the right posterior parahippocampal gyrus is important for the development of long-term recollection for high-complexity scenes. Similarly, Maril et al. [47] studied children (age 8–11) and young adults with an fMRI paradigm consisting of noun/color combinations. The adults recruited left PFC and parietal and occipital-temporal cortices, while the children recruited the right occipital cortex, providing evidence that episodic encoding for children depends more on perceptual systems, whereas top-down frontal control and parietal structures are involved more with age.

The complexity of understanding memory is also in part due to the complexity of the hippocampus. The hippocampus can be divided longitudinally (anterior, body, posterior) and by subregions (CA fields, dentate gyrus). The complexity of the subregions of the hippocampus is an important variable to be considered and an area of current focus in the field, which is beyond the scope of this review. Some studies segmenting the hippocampus longitudinally will be briefly reviewed here. In typical development, structural and functional developmental changes occur along the longitudinal axis of the hippocampus. Structurally, decreased anterior and increased posterior hippocampus volume occurs from age 4 to 25 (more pronounced on left) [48]. Another study found a positive relationship between episodic memory and anterior hippocampal volume in both the left and right hemispheres for 6-year-old children but not for 4-year-old children, indicating varying, dynamic changes in anterior and posterior volume throughout development [49]. Functionally, episodic recall is associated with activity in the posterior hippocampus in TD children and anteriorly in TD adults [50]. A more recent study by Sastre et al. [51] found that good memory performance in adults was associated with more focal hippocampal recruitment (hippocampal head) compared to high-performing 10–11-year-olds and low-performing adults where activation was observed across the entire hippocampus. Thus, even within this hippocampal structure, there are subcomponents that may have functional differences over development.

Epilepsy

For both children and adults with epilepsy, reliable pre-surgical guides are necessary to predict and minimize the adverse impact of temporal resection on memory functioning. Neuropsychological assessment and the intracarotid amobarbital procedure (Wada) can be used for this purpose, but the former is not highly predictive of postoperative outcome in children [37] and the latter is invasive and challenging to perform in children. Transition to a reliable, non-invasive procedure, as in language mapping, reduces risk for morbidity and extends the lower age limit for studies. fMRI is one promising alternative for assessing memory functioning pre-surgically; however, memory paradigms have been difficult to use clinically. One challenge is designing paradigms that increase cerebral blood flow sufficiently to detect MTL/hippocampal signal on an individual basis because memory processing pathways are always “online” [52]. Another challenge is that the hippocampus is difficult to image due to location-based vulnerability to susceptibility artifacts. Despite these difficulties, many group-level fMRI memory studies have been conducted in adults with epilepsy (see Table 2) [14, 20, 41, 53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68]. Few of these studies have employed the paradigms common in TD adults, such as the Remember/Know paradigms (for review, see Rugg and Vilberg [39]). This is likely due to the complexity of these paradigms, which can be challenging for patients with any cognitive difficulties. Further, the majority of epilepsy studies have only conducted fMRI during memory encoding, with recognition or recall testing after scanning (see Table 2).

Table 1 fMRI memory studies for TD children

Several of these studies show that some adults with TLE demonstrate greater activation in MTL contralateral to the seizure focus (e.g., for left TLE, verbal encoding yields activation in right MTL) [41, 42, 69]. Memory fMRI can also predict postoperative memory outcomes for adults with TLE. Models of adult postoperative functioning propose that these outcomes are predicted either by activation in the hippocampus to be resected (hippocampal adequacy) or the non-resected hippocampus (hippocampal reserve) [58]. Adults with refractory TLE demonstrate greater memory decline following temporal resection with greater ipsilateral compared to contralateral MTL activation on preoperative fMRI memory, supporting hippocampal adequacy [20, 56, 59, 62, 70]. Research also shows that greater pre- than postoperative ipsilateral posterior MTL activation is related to better verbal memory after left anterior temporal lobe resection [67]. These results provide evidence that the capacity of the posterior remnant of the ipsilateral hippocampus is important for postoperative verbal memory functioning, not the functional reserve of the contralateral hippocampus or early postoperative reorganization to ipsilateral posterior MTL.

Most of the fMRI memory studies that have been conducted in adults with epilepsy are group-level studies (see Table 2) [14, 20, 41, 53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68]. Two of those studies have also shown that memory fMRI paradigms can predict postoperative memory outcome with individual fMRI results [61, 68]. Frings et al. [61] studied 22 patients with mesial TLE using a spatial memory task in a 3D virtual environment. They found that hippocampal activation lateralization during this task was significantly correlated with postoperative decline in verbal memory, such that greater activation of the to-be-resected hippocampus was related to increased verbal memory decline. Sidhu et al. [68] studied 50 patients with TLE and 26 controls with an fMRI memory word list encoding paradigm. Results showed that left lateralization within a medial temporal and frontal mask could predict verbal memory postsurgical outcome for individual patients, while bilateral posterior hippocampal activation correlated with less verbal memory decline.

As a result of this work, recent AAN Practice Guidelines indicate moderate (Level B) evidence for memory fMRI. However, to our knowledge, no prior fMRI studies of memory functioning in pediatric TLE have been published; thus, this is an important area of future investigation. In Sepeta et al. [71], we measured hippocampal activation indirectly via a language fMRI task in TD children and adults, as well as those with focal epilepsy (see Fig. 3). We found a developmental difference in MTL lateralization such that MTL activation was left lateralized for adults, but less so in children. Each individual’s peak voxel within the bilateral MTL ROIs illustrates that adults have more left-lateralized activation than children. Language lateralization (Broca’s and Wernicke’s) predicted 19% of the variance in MTL lateralization for adults, but not for children. These results suggested a developmental shift in lateralization of MTL function, with increased left lateralization across the age span. This shift may help explain why children have better memory outcomes following resection compared to adults. Children may initially engage both MTLs, with specialization for specific types of material (verbal—left MTL, visual—right MTL) occurring later in development. This follows research demonstrating increased left lateralization for language [72] and right lateralization for visuospatial functions with age [73]. For children with epilepsy who undergo surgery, these cognitive functions may be preserved due to persistence of (earlier) developmental processing capacity in contralateral homologs (Developmental Origins Hypothesis).

Table 2 fMRI memory studies for adults with epilepsy
Fig. 3
figure 3

During language fMRI, the peak MTL voxel is depicted for each individual in the adult and pediatric age group (blue represents TD controls and green represents epilepsy). MTL was more left lateralized in adults than children. MTL, mesial temporal lobe. Adapted with permission from Sepeta et al. [71]

Resting-state connectivity

Typical development

Resting-state connectivity involves a “task-free” MRI paradigm while patients are at rest, while remaining awake and alert. Resting-state fMRI does not require specific tasks to assess for different functions (e.g., motor, language), and many networks can be tested simultaneously, saving scanning time. It is less demanding than task-based fMRI and thus can be used in patients with neurological and cognitive deficits, who might be precluded from participating in task-based fMRI. Resting-state has been found to be reliable across imaging sessions [74, 75] and different study participants [75, 76]. However, there are some disadvantages, which include that the physiological basis of the resting-state signal is not completely understood and the state is unmonitored; therefore, information about the cognitive processes during resting state (e.g., inner speech) is not available (for review, see Goodyear et al. [77]).

Memory involves a distributed neuroanatomical network, and hippocampal connectivity to this network is an indicator of episodic memory function. In TD adult resting-state functional connectivity (FC) studies, a memory network has been identified between the hippocampus and medial PFC, lateral temporal and several parietal regions (including posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), retrosplenial cortex, and bilateral inferior parietal lobule (IPL)) [78]. Specific regions in this network are associated with successful memory recall, such as the PFC, IPL, and the medial surface extending from the retrosplenial cortex to PCC.

In typical development, many resting-state networks have been identified as early as 26 weeks gestation [79]. Resting-state studies of typical development in early and middle childhood have revealed network patterns common in children [80,81,82,83]. Many of the resting-state networks in TD children are similar to adults, but consistent differences include decreased long-range and increased short-range correlations [84]. A recent study of typical development in 4- and 6-year-olds found similar hippocampal FC to adults [85]. The regions connected to the hippocampus included the lateral temporal regions, precuneus, and multiple parietal and prefrontal regions. The strength of posterior hippocampal FC with the right middle temporal gyrus was associated with memory ability. Another study of children aged 4–10 found an increase over age in strength of hippocampal connectivity with lateral temporal lobes and the anterior cingulate [86]. Age-controlled analyses showed similar hippocampal–parietal memory network identified in adults.

Epilepsy

For adults with TLE, several resting-state studies have shown mixed results. Some find a reduction in hippocampal FC either ipsilateral to the seizure focus [69, 87] or bilaterally [88, 89] in adults with TLE. Alternatively, some find strong ipsilateral hippocampal FC [90]. Haneef et al. [91] found both increased and decreased hippocampal FC, with increased FC to the limbic network (temporal lobe, insula, thalamus), frontal and angular gyri, basal ganglia, brainstem, and cerebellum, and reduced hippocampal FC with the sensorimotor cortex and default mode network. Overall, the left TLE group often shows more marked FC changes than right TLE [92].

Hippocampal FC during resting state has not been studied systematically with a pediatric TLE population. Some studies of FC in children with focal epilepsy have been conducted using resting-state fMRI. Vaessen et al. [93] found that children with frontal lobe epilepsy demonstrated higher modularity than controls, indicating that subnetworks were underconnected in epilepsy. Overall, patients demonstrated abnormal modular organization, with a decrease in long-range and increase in interhemispheric connectivity in patients. Cognition (performance and reaction time on a computerized visual searching task) was associated with higher modularity scores for patients. Besseling et al. [94] studied children with Rolandic epilepsy and found reduced FC between the left sensorimotor area and right inferior frontal gyrus in patients. FC between those two regions was positively correlated with language scores for the patients.

In sum, resting-state fMRI is a promising method for patients with neurological and cognitive deficits. For TLE, many resting-state studies have focused on hippocampal FC with adults and, these have shown mixed findings; however, more research is necessary for both typical development and pediatric TLE.

Conclusion

In summary, investigation of memory functioning with advanced neuroimaging has implications for developmental neuroscience, as well as clinical practice. Our understanding of the basic neuroscience concept of neural plasticity has already benefited from studies of language reorganization in epilepsy, showing atypical language lateralization is more prevalent in patients with early seizure onset [95]. However, little is known about processes facilitating memory reorganization, and there is controversy about the degree to which function is preserved or impaired due to reorganization. Aside from the lack of neuroimaging memory studies in children with TLE, studies in TD children are limited. Important differences exist between children and adults in epilepsy and the effects of temporal resection on memory networks, suggesting a specific window for memory plasticity. It is plausible that children use both hippocampi in memory encoding and retrieval, and that across typical development hippocampi become material-specific. If true, this would have a significant impact on our understanding of material specificity in memory, as well as epilepsy interventions, such as surgical resection. More neuroimaging studies of memory are necessary, as studies of typical development are limited and there is a lack of neuroimaging memory studies in children with TLE. Resting-state studies can provide meaningful information regarding the development of memory systems, but more research is also required using this technique. Further study of memory via both task-based and resting-state fMRI for TD and children with TLE is crucial to noninvasively ascertain the developmental trajectory of postoperative risk for memory decline.

References

  1. Markowitsch HJ. The anatomical bases of memory. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge: MIT press; 1999. p. 781–96.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Kandel ER, Kupfermann I, Inverson S. Learning and memory. In: Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM, editors. Principles of Neural Science. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2000. p. 1227–46.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Ghetti S, Bunge S. Neural changes underlying the development of episodic memory during middle childhood. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2012;2(4):381–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Eldridge LL, Engel SA, Zeineh MM, Bookheimer SY, Knowlton BJ. A dissociation of encoding and retrieval processes in the human hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3280–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Wiebe S. Epidemiology of temporal lobe epilepsy. Can J Neurol Sci. 2000;27(Suppl 1):S6–10 discussion S20–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bell BD, Davies KG. Anterior temporal lobectomy, hippocampal sclerosis, and memory: recent neuropsychological findings. Neuropsychol Rev. 1998;8:25–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Helmstaedter C, Grunwald T, Lehnertz K, Gleißner U, Elger CE. Differential involvement of left Temporolateral and Temporomesial structures in verbal declarative learning and memory: evidence from temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Cogn. 1997;35:110–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Helmstaedter C, Elger CE. Chronic temporal lobe epilepsy: a neurodevelopmental or progressively dementing disease? Brain. 2009;132:2822–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kim H, Yi S, Son EI, Kim J. Material-specific memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: effects of seizure laterality and language dominance. Neuropsychol. 2003;17:59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Saling MM. Verbal memory in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: beyond material specificity. Brain. 2009;132:570–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Castro L, Silva L, Adda C, Banaskiwitz N, Xavier A, Jorge C, Valerio R, Nitrini R. Low prevalence but high specificity of material-specific memory impairment in epilepsy associated with hippocampal sclerosis. Epilepsia. 2013;54(10):1735–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Engel JJ. Outcome with respect to epileptic seizure. In: Engel JJ, editor. Surgical Treatment of the Epilepsies. New York: Raven Press; 1993. p. 609–21.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Wyllie E, et al. Seizure outcome after epilepsy surgery in children and adolescents. Ann Neurol. 1998;44:740–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Bonelli SB, et al. Imaging memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: predicting the effects of temporal lobe resection. Brain. 2010;133:1186–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Henke K, et al. Functional neuroimaging predicts individual memory outcome after amygdalohippocampectomy. Neuroreport. 2003;14:1197–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hermann BP, Seidenberg M, Haltiner A, Wyler AR. Relationship of age at onset, chronologic age, and adequacy of preoperative performance to verbal memory change after anterior temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia. 1995;36:137–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Janszky J, et al. Temporal lobe epilepsy with hippocampal sclerosis: predictors for long-term surgical outcome. Brain. 2005;128:395–404.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Powell HW, et al. Imaging language pathways predicts postoperative naming deficits. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79:327–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Gleissner U, et al. Pre- and postoperative verbal memory in pediatric patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res. 2002;51:287–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Janszky J, et al. Functional MRI predicts memory performance after right mesiotemporal epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia. 2005;46:244–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Binder JR, Swanson SJ, Hammeke TA, Sabsevitz DS. A comparison of five fMRI protocols for mapping speech comprehension systems. Epilepsia. 2008;49:1980–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Elliott IM, Lach L, Smith ML. I just want to be normal: a qualitative study exploring how children and adolescents view the impact of intractable epilepsy on their quality of life. Epilepsy Behav EB. 2005;7:664–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Arunkumar G, Wyllie E, Kotagal P, Ong HT, Gilliam F. Parent- and patient-validated content for pediatric epilepsy quality-of-life assessment. Epilepsia. 2000;41:1474–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kadis DS, Stollstorff M, Elliott I, Lach L, Smith ML. Cognitive and psychological predictors of everyday memory in children with intractable epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2004;5:37–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Dam M. Children with epilepsy: the effect of seizures, syndromes, and etiological factors on cognitive functioning. Epilepsia. 1990;31(Suppl 4):S26–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Menlove L, Reilly C. Memory in children with epilepsy: a systematic review. Seizure. 2015;25:126–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Guimaraes CA, et al. Temporal lobe epilepsy in childhood: comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. J Child Neurol. 2007;22:836–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hermann BP, Seidenberg M, Bell B. The neurodevelopmental impact of childhood onset temporal lobe epilepsy on brain structure and function and the risk of progressive cognitive effects. Prog Brain Res. 2002;135:429–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Jambaque I, Dellatolas G, Dulac O, Ponsot G, Signoret JL. Verbal and visual memory impairment in children with epilepsy. Neuropsychologia. 1993;31:1321–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Bender HA, Marks BC, Brown ER, Zach L, Zaroff CM. Neuropsychologic performance of children with epilepsy on the NEPSY. Pediatr Neurol. 2007;36:312–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Williams J, et al. Patterns of memory performance in children with controlled epilepsy on the CVLT-C. Child Neuropsychol. 2001;7:15–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Cohen M. Auditory/verbal and visual/spatial memory in children with complex partial epilepsy of temporal lobe origin. Brain Cogn. 1992;20:315–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Gonzalez LM, Anderson VA, Wood SJ, Mitchell LA, Harvey AS. The localization and lateralization of memory deficits in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2007;48:124–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Lendt M, Helmstaedter C, Elger CE. Pre- and postoperative neuropsychological profiles in children and adolescents with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. 1999;40:1543–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Cormack F, Vargha-Khadem F, Wood SJ, Cross JH, Baldeweg T. Memory in paediatric temporal lobe epilepsy: effects of lesion type and side. Epilepsy Res. 2012;98:255–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Kernan CL, et al. Neurocognitive profiles in children with epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2012;53:2156–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Lah S. Neuropsychological outcome following focal cortical removal for intractable epilepsy in children. Epilepsy Behav. 2004;5:804–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Law N, Benifla M, Rutka J, Smith ML. Verbal memory after temporal lobe epilepsy surgery in children: do only mesial structures matter? Epilepsia. 2017;58:291–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Rugg MD, Vilberg KL. Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23:255–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Eldridge LL, Knowlton BJ, Furmanski CS, Bookheimer SY, Engel SA. Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1149–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Golby AJ, et al. Memory lateralization in medial temporal lobe epilepsy assessed by functional MRI. Epilepsia. 2002;43:855–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Kelley WM, et al. Hemispheric specialization in human dorsal frontal cortex and medial temporal lobe for verbal and nonverbal memory encoding. Neuron. 1998;20:927–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Ofen N, et al. Development of the declarative memory system in the human brain. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1198–205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. DeMaster D, Pathman T, Ghetti S. Development of memory for spatial context: hippocampal and cortical contributions. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:2415–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Ghetti S, DeMaster DM, Yonelinas AP, Bunge SA. Developmental differences in medial temporal lobe function during memory encoding. J Neurosci. 2010;30:9548–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Chai XJ, Ofen N, Jacobs LF, Gabrieli JD. Scene complexity: influence on perception, memory, and development in the medial temporal lobe. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;4:21.

  47. Maril A, et al. Event congruency and episodic encoding: a developmental fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:3036–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Gogtay N, et al. Dynamic mapping of normal human hippocampal development. Hippocampus. 2006;16:664–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Riggins T, Blankenship SL, Mulligan E, Rice K, Redcay E. Developmental differences in relations between episodic memory and hippocampal subregion volume during early childhood. Child Dev. 2015;86:1710–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Demaster DM, Ghetti S. Developmental differences in hippocampal and cortical contributions to episodic retrieval. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav. 2013;49:1482–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Sastre M, Wendelken C, Lee JK, Bunge SA, Ghetti S. Age- and performance-related differences in hippocampal contributions to episodic retrieval. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016;19:42–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Gabrieli JD, Brewer JB, Desmond JE, Glover GH. Separate neural bases of two fundamental memory processes in the human medial temporal lobe. Science. 1997;276:264–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Detre JA, et al. Functional MRI lateralization of memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology. 1998;50:926–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Killgore WD, et al. Functional MRI and the Wada test provide complementary information for predicting post-operative seizure control. Seizure J Br Epilepsy Assoc. 1999;8:450–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Dupont S, et al. Episodic memory in left temporal lobe epilepsy: a functional MRI study. Brain. 2000;123:1722–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Jokeit H, Okujava M, Woermann FG. Memory fMRI lateralizes temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology. 2001;57:1786–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Richardson MP, Strange BA, Duncan JS, Dolan RJ. Preserved verbal memory function in left medial temporal pathology involves reorganisation of function to right medial temporal lobe. NeuroImage. 2003;20:S112–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Rabin ML. Functional MRI predicts post-surgical memory following temporal lobectomy. Brain. 2004;127:2286–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Richardson MP, Strange BA, Duncan JS, Dolan RJ. Memory fMRI in left hippocampal sclerosis: optimizing the approach to predicting postsurgical memory. Neurology. 2006;66:699–705.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Avila C, et al. Memory lateralization with 2 functional MR imaging tasks in patients with lesions in the temporal lobe. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2006;27:498–503.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Frings L, et al. Lateralization of hippocampal activation differs between left and right temporal lobe epilepsy patients and correlates with postsurgical verbal learning decrement. Epilepsy Res. 2008;78:161–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Powell HWR, et al. Preoperative fMRI predicts memory decline following anterior temporal lobe resection. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;79:686–93.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Binder J, et al. A comparison of two fMRI methods for predicting verbal memory decline after left temporal lobectomy_Language lateralization versus hippocampal activation asymmetry. Epilepsia. 2010;51:618–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Dupont S, et al. Functional MR imaging or Wada test: which is the better predictor of individual postoperative memory outcome? Radiology. 2010;255:128–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Alessio A, et al. Brain plasticity for verbal and visual memories in patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy and hippocampal sclerosis: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34:186–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Sidhu MK, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study mapping the episodic memory encoding network in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain J Neurol. 2013;136:1868–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Bonelli SB, et al. Memory reorganization following anterior temporal lobe resection: a longitudinal functional MRI study. Brain. 2013;136:1889–900.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Sidhu MK, et al. Memory fMRI predicts verbal memory decline after anterior temporal lobe resection. Neurology. 2015;84:1512–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. James GA, Tripathi SP, Ojemann JG, Gross RE, Drane DL. Diminished default mode network recruitment of the hippocampus and parahippocampus in temporal lobe epilepsy: clinical article. J Neurosurg. 2013;119:288–300.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Richardson MP. Pre-operative verbal memory fMRI predicts post-operative memory decline after left temporal lobe resection. Brain. 2004;127:2419–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Sepeta LN, et al. Age-dependent mesial temporal lobe lateralization in language fMRI. Epilepsia. 2016;57:122–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Berl MM, et al. Regional differences in the developmental trajectory of lateralization of the language network. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:270–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Everts R, et al. Strengthening of laterality of verbal and visuospatial functions during childhood and adolescence. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:473–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Biswal BB, et al. Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:4734–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Shehzad Z, et al. The resting brain: unconstrained yet reliable. Cereb Cortex. 2009;1991(19):2209–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Damoiseaux JS, et al. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:13848–53.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Goodyear B, Liebenthal E, Mosher V. Active and passive fMRI for presurgical mapping of motor and language cortex. In: Papageorgiou TD, Christopoulos GI, Smirnakis SM, editors. Advanced brain neuroimaging topics in health and disease - methods and applications. London: InTech; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  78. Vincent JL, et al. Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal-parietal memory network. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:3517–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Smyser CD, et al. Longitudinal analysis of neural network development in preterm infants. Cereb Cortex. 2010;1991(20):2852–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Fair DA, et al. Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:13507–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Fair DA, et al. The maturing architecture of the brain’s default network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:4028–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Kelly AMC, et al. Development of anterior cingulate functional connectivity from late childhood to early adulthood. Cereb Cortex. 2009;1991(19):640–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Thomason ME, et al. Default-mode function and task-induced deactivation have overlapping brain substrates in children. NeuroImage. 2008;41:1493–503.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Lee MH, Smyser CD, Shimony JS. Resting-state fMRI: a review of methods and clinical applications. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2013;34:1866–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Riggins T, Geng F, Blankenship SL, Redcay E. Hippocampal functional connectivity and episodic memory in early childhood. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2016;19:58–69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Blankenship SL, Redcay E, Dougherty LR, Riggins T. Development of hippocampal functional connectivity during childhood: hippocampal functional connectivity development. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38:182–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Pereira FR, et al. Asymmetrical hippocampal connectivity in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: evidence from resting state fMRI. BMC Neurosci. 2010;11:66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Liao W, et al. Altered functional connectivity and small-world in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. PLoS One. 2010;5:e8525.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  89. Zhang X, et al. Social network theory applied to resting-state fMRI connectivity data in the identification of epilepsy networks with iterative feature selection. J Neurosci Methods. 2011;199:129–39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Wagner K, et al. Hippocampal functional connectivity reflects verbal episodic memory network integrity. Neuroreport. 2007;18:1719–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Haneef Z. Functional neuro-imaging as a pre-surgical tool in epilepsy. Ann Indian Acad Neurol. 2014;17:56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Doucet G, Osipowicz K, Sharan A, Sperling MR, Tracy JI. Extratemporal functional connectivity impairments at rest are related to memory performance in mesial temporal epilepsy. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34:2202–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Vaessen MJ, et al. Abnormal modular organization of functional networks in cognitively impaired children with frontal lobe epilepsy. Cereb Cortex. 2013;1991(23):1997–2006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Besseling RMH, et al. Reduced functional integration of the sensorimotor and language network in rolandic epilepsy. NeuroImage Clin. 2013;2:239–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Gaillard WD, et al. Atypical language in lesional and nonlesional complex partial epilepsy. Neurology. 2007;69:1761–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Golby A. Material-specific lateralization in the medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex during memory encoding. Brain. 2001;124(9):1841–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Menon V, Boyett-Anderson J, Reiss A. Maturation of medial temporal lobe response and connectivity during memory encoding. Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25(1):379–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  98. Maril A, Davis P, Koo J, Reggev N, Zuckerman M, Ehrenfeld L, Mulkern R, Waber D, Rivkin M. Developmental fMRI study of episodic verbal memory encoding in children. Neurology. 2010;75(23):2110–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Maria Z. Chroneos and all of the families and children who participated in our studies. We also thank Ms. Madeleine Anna Isaacson who arrived into this world in time to help her mom (L.N.S.) complete the paper.

Funding

This publication was supported by Award Number 1U54HD090257 from the NIH, District of Columbia Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center Award (DC-IDDRC) program. This work was also supported by the Susan S. Spencer Clinical Research Training Fellowship [to L.N.S.]; Avery Translational Research Career Development Program Award [through the Clinical and Translational Science Institute at Children’s National (CTSI-CN) to L.N.S.]; the National Institutes of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, NIH [1K23NS093152-01A1 to L.N.S., 5K23NS065121-01A2 to M.M.B., R01NS44280 to W.D.G.]; NICHD Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center and Children’s National Health System Grant [P30HD040677]. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the District of Columbia Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center or the National Institutes of Health.

Availability of data and materials

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LNS reviewed the past literature and wrote the manuscript. WDG and MMB contributed to the editing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Leigh N. Sepeta.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sepeta, L.N., Berl, M.M. & Gaillard, W.D. Imaging episodic memory during development and childhood epilepsy. J Neurodevelop Disord 10, 40 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-018-9255-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-018-9255-8