Skip to main content

Social attention during object engagement: toward a cross-species measure of preferential social orienting

Abstract

Background

A central challenge in preclinical research investigating the biology of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is the translation of ASD-related social phenotypes across humans and animal models. Social orienting, an observable, evolutionarily conserved behavior, represents a promising cross-species ASD phenotype given that disrupted social orienting is an early-emerging ASD feature with evidence for predicting familial recurrence. Here, we adapt a competing-stimulus social orienting task from domesticated dogs to naturalistic play behavior in human toddlers and test whether this approach indexes decreased social orienting in ASD.

Methods

Play behavior was coded from the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) in two samples of toddlers, each with and without ASD. Sample 1 (n = 16) consisted of community-ascertained research participants, while Sample 2 involved a prospective study of infants at a high or low familial liability for ASD (n = 67). Coding quantified the child’s looks towards the experimenter and caregiver, a social stimulus, while playing with high-interest toys, a non-social stimulus. A competing-stimulus measure of “Social Attention During Object Engagement” (SADOE) was calculated by dividing the number of social looks by total time spent playing with toys. SADOE was compared based on ASD diagnosis and differing familial liability for ASD.

Results

In both samples, toddlers with ASD exhibited significantly lower SADOE compared to toddlers without ASD, with large effect sizes (Hedges’ g ≥ 0.92) driven by a lower frequency of child-initiated spontaneous looks. Among toddlers at high familial likelihood of ASD, toddlers with ASD showed lower SADOE than toddlers without ASD, while SADOE did not differ based on presence or absence of familial ASD risk alone. SADOE correlated negatively with ADOS social affect calibrated severity scores and positively with the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales social subscale. In a binary logistic regression model, SADOE alone correctly classified 74.1% of cases, which rose to 85.2% when combined with cognitive development.

Conclusions

This work suggests that a brief behavioral measure pitting a high-interest nonsocial stimulus against the innate draw of social partners can serve as a feasible cross-species measure of social orienting, with implications for genetically informative behavioral phenotyping of social deficits in ASD and other neurodevelopmental disorders.

Background

Opportunities for engagement with the social world present soon after birth. During neurotypical development, infants preferentially orient toward social stimuli in their environment, showing greater attention to faces and face-like patterns [1, 2] and preference for faces with direct rather than averted gaze [3]. Instances of preferential social orienting, which emerge as more reflexive behaviors during infancy, prompt adult caregivers to respond contingently with facial expressions and sounds that provide the infant with early social communicative experiences [4]. This reflexive infant orienting transitions to volitional gaze toward faces, allowing infants to experience the opportunity to control the contingency between orienting and caregiver response [4]. As an early-emerging social attentional bias, social orienting is theorized to represent an aspect of social motivation, the drive that compels species toward affiliative and cooperative behaviors [5] and is hypothesized to provide a foundation for learning skills contributing to later social communication, such as joint attention [6, 7]. In keeping with this hypothesis, children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental condition clinically defined by social communication deficits, show characteristic differences in social orienting, including reduced eye contact and response to name as well as impaired joint attention prior to diagnosis [7,8,9,10,11].

A large body of eye-tracking literature supports that in ASD, divergence in the development of social attentional bias from a neurotypical trajectory occurs prior to toddlerhood, when ASD can first be diagnosed. Objective quantification of social orienting using eye-tracking while children view dynamic video clips has found a decline in social orienting to eyes within the first year of life among infants who develop ASD [12]. Similarly, toddlers with ASD spend less time attending to social stimuli like eyes and faces compared to controls [13, 14], and social orienting has been shown to correlate with social competence in children with ASD [15]. Contextual factors influencing social orienting in children with ASD have been clarified by competing-stimulus eye-tracking paradigms, which present social and nonsocial visual stimuli simultaneously and compare the proportion of time spent looking at each stimulus type [16,17,18]. These studies have observed that although young children with ASD, like controls, generally orient toward people and items in their visual field, their gaze towards social stimuli is more strongly reduced in the presence of social bids for attention (e.g., directed speech and eye contact) and more salient nonsocial items related to intense or restricted interests [16, 18]. These competing-stimulus eye-tracking metrics of social orienting have shown utility as a predictive marker of ASD and ASD severity [17, 19,20,21].

Naturalistic tasks relying on competing-stimulus approaches have also been informative for demonstrating how simultaneous information in the environment can highlight relative deficits in social orienting for children with versus without ASD. In one experiment, when toddlers were given a tablet displaying video clips, toddlers with ASD were less likely to re-orient towards an experimenter calling their name compared to neurotypically developing or developmentally delayed toddlers [22]. In another experiment, six-month-old infants with high familial liability for ASD made fewer spontaneous looks towards a caregiver than controls when presented with a musical toy [23]. These findings align with observations from competing-stimulus eye-tracking studies demonstrating reduced social orienting in the context of greater attention to nonsocial stimuli among subjects with ASD compared to neurotypically developing controls [18, 24].

Social orienting represents a promising ASD biomarker given it both indexes individual differences relevant to ASD symptoms [19] and is genetically influenced [25,26,27]; moreover, decreased social orienting in infants may serve as a predictive marker of ASD recurrence in infants with a family history of ASD [7]. Recent evidence supports that both ASD [28] and social orienting [29] are also subject to epigenetic regulation, whereby experiences lead to modifications of the genome that alter gene expression but not the DNA sequence. In infants, profiles of DNA methylation, a common epigenetic modification, have been associated with subsequent ASD diagnoses [30] and ASD polygenic risk scores [31]. Characterization of genetic, epigenetic, and neural correlates of social orienting, including those identifiable prior to ASD diagnosis, may thus elucidate mechanisms contributing to social communication impairment in ASD and the development of affiliative behaviors more generally.

In humans, however, studies of how genetic and epigenetic factors influence neurodevelopment, as well as how early-emerging capacities like social orienting may promote the development of social communication, are constrained by practical and ethical limitations on monitoring and controlling children’s naturalistic environments and probing underlying neural circuitry. Preclinical animal models, which are amenable to testing hypotheses that integrate the impact of genetics and environment on evolutionarily conserved elements of social development, therefore provide a valuable approach for characterizing the biology of ASD. Recent work on chimpanzee and macaque adaptations of the Social Responsiveness Scale, a human measure of quantitative autistic traits, has demonstrated the feasibility of cross-species phenotyping of ASD-relevant social behaviors [32, 33]. Among social behaviors affected in ASD, social orienting is a promising target for cross-species phenotyping given that it is evolutionarily conserved in affiliative species such as chicks, nonhuman primates, and dogs, which like humans, show a preference for faces of conspecifics [34,35,36] and biological motion [37,38,39]. Eye-tracking studies in young macaques have also found that attention to social stimuli predicts later social abilities [40], analogous to observed relationships in humans between social orienting and social competence [15], and is diminished by ASD-associated perturbations such as maternal immune activation [41]. Brain regions responsible for processing social stimuli such as biological motion are similar between humans and nonhuman primates [42], supporting the translational relevance of animal models to explore neural underpinnings of social orienting. However, cross-species measures of social orienting are currently limited given the challenge of developing behavioral assays that are comparable between humans and other animals. Factors contributing to this challenge include less elaborated social-cognitive abilities, e.g., in rodents, as well as an evolutionary history of distinct selective pressures within disparate environmental and social contexts for other mammalian species, e.g., non-human primates [43].

The domestic dog, or canis familiaris, has emerged as a tractable animal model for ASD [43]. Increasing evidence suggests that the co-evolution of humans and canines entailed in domestication has allowed adaptations in socio-cognitive functioning and selection of social traits, such as high responsiveness and reward sensitivity to human social signals, that have resulted in interspecific social competence between domestic dogs and humans [43, 44]. Because domestic dogs have been encultured in human society, evolutionary changes in canine genetic architecture, as well as epigenetic processes prompted by shared environment, may exhibit convergence with the biology underlying social orienting in humans. Human-directed sociability and sensitivity to human communication exhibit heritable influences in domestic dogs [45, 46], which display a variety of human-analog social abilities during canine-human interactions including sensitivity to human emotional facial expressions [47], initiation of eye contact [48], gaze following [49], and production of referential cues [50]. While several of these behaviors also occur in non-human primates, humans’ evolutionarily closest animal relative, in domestic dogs, they are distinguished by greater sophistication and flexibility [44, 51]. Unlike non-human primates, dogs have been shown to integrate visual and auditory cues to discriminate positive and negative emotions in both conspecifics and humans [52], to infer referents from human gaze [49, 53], and to locate hidden food based on a variety of human social communicative cues [54].

Recent findings have also shown overlapping neurobiological underpinnings for social behavior in humans and human-oriented social behavior in domestic dogs. For example, the amygdala and insula, substrates of affective processing in humans, have been implicated in neural responses to salient human social stimuli in domestic dogs [55]. Like humans, dogs demonstrate increased social responsiveness to humans when exposed to oxytocin, a neuropeptide hormone that enhances affiliative behavior in vertebrates [56]. Increased DNA methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene has been associated with ASD [57] and lower neural responsiveness to social stimuli in humans [29], as well as reduced approach towards unfamiliar humans in dogs [58]. These parallels in phenomenology and mechanism highlight the unique viability of canine behavioral models to measure ASD-relevant traits that may share common genetic, epigenetic, or neural mechanisms with ASD.

Dogs’ coevolutionary history with humans also suggests that naturalistic studies of social behavior in domestic dogs can be conducted based on dogs’ interactions with humans [59], as demonstrated by human-directed sociability tasks [60,61,62,63]. In these paradigms, domestic dogs have shown preferential orienting towards humans which can vary according to familiarity with the experimenter [62, 63], the use of human social cues such as pointing [61], and prior actions of a human partner [64]. This profile of social attention to humans corresponds to behavior observed in neurotypical human infants and young children [43, 44, 51], lending additional support to the applicability of these canine tasks to early childhood studies of altered social orienting in neurodevelopmental conditions such as ASD. Consistent with this notion, a recent competing-stimulus index of the degree to which dogs oriented towards a human social partner versus a highly engaging nonsocial distractor was genetically informative, as it associated with structural variants in several genes within the canine ortholog of the Williams-Beuren Syndrome locus. Within this region, duplication in humans leads to neurodevelopmental syndrome featuring autistic-like behavior, whereas deletion is associated with hypersociability, suggesting a dosage-related effect on social function [62]. This genetic finding implies the opportunity to leverage a competing-stimulus approach, which has anchored metrics of social orienting in both human infants (via eye-tracking studies) and canines, as a comparative cross-species metric of social orienting. Such a metric could ultimately provide a tool to identify evolutionarily conserved neural and genetic correlates of this behavioral domain, including those implicated in ASD, as well as disentangle downstream social learning opportunities, and potential associated epigenetic processes, that may contribute to the development of ASD.

In the present study, we developed and tested a competing-stimulus measure, “Social Attention During Object Engagement” (SADOE), which we modeled on the genetically informative competing-stimulus index of social orienting developed in canines [62]. To investigate whether SADOE could distinguish toddlers with and without ASD, we quantified SADOE in two samples, a group of community-ascertained toddler research participants with and without ASD and a prospective study of infant siblings at high and low familial liability for ASD. SADOE was quantified in the context of free play, a naturalistic activity enhancing ecological validity, which is incorporated within the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), a semi-structured diagnostic assessment for ASD [65, 66]. To index SADOE, we isolated segments of play in which the child engaged with pre-selected high-interest toys and then coded the frequency of the child's social looks during this period. We hypothesized that, in agreement with previous human and canine studies, the presence of a salient nonsocial object competing with the attentional draw of a social partner would provide sufficient dynamic range to capture individual differences in social orienting and that SADOE would be reduced among toddlers with ASD as well as toddlers with elevated familial ASD liability.

Methods

Participants

Sample 1 – community-ascertained sample of toddlers with ASD

SADOE coding procedures were initially tested in a subsample of children with (n = 8) or without ASD (n = 8, see Table 1 for demographics) who were enrolled in a longitudinal research studies on the development of ASD and ASD traits [67, 68]. Comparison participants without ASD included toddler twins from the general population, who were epidemiologically ascertained using the Missouri Family Register, a statewide population-representative twin birth registry [67]. Toddlers with ASD were identified based on either having a community diagnosis or suspected diagnosis of ASD. Research confirmation of a clinical-best-estimate ASD diagnosis was obtained using the ADOS conducted by research-reliable assessors. Participants included in analyses had valid and codable video-recorded ADOS evaluations performed between age 18 months and 3 years.

Table 1 Sample characteristics

Sample 2 – prospective infant sibling study of ASD

ADOS videos from a subsample of toddlers (n = 67) enrolled at the Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS) at the Washington University in St. Louis site [69] were coded (see Table 1 for demographics). IBIS is a prospective study of infant siblings at low and high familial liability for ASD based on having an older sibling with ASD. Given the established high heritability of ASD, familial liability is likely to reflect the influence of inherited genetic factors for ASD [70]. The family study design of IBIS allowed participants to be classified into three groups at differing likelihood of having an ASD diagnosis, listed here in ascending order of familial ASD liability: 1) the low-likelihood negative group (LL-; n = 19), comprised of infant siblings without an ASD diagnosis or a first- or second-degree family history of ASD, 2) the high-likelihood negative group (HL-; n = 33), comprised of infant siblings without an ASD diagnosis who had an older sibling with ASD, and 3) the high-likelihood positive group (HL + ; n = 15), comprised of infant siblings with an ASD diagnosis and an older sibling with ASD. Selected participants had a valid, codable video recording of the ADOS conducted at the 24-month study visit. A clinical-best-estimate diagnosis of ASD was made by an experienced examiner based on all available information from the 24-month IBIS behavioral battery, including the ADOS and observations during in-person assessments. Testing, video, and interview data were reviewed by a second experienced experimenter to confirm that criteria for an ASD (Autism or Pervasive Developmental Disorder NOS) were met using the DSM-IV-TR checklist at 24 months.

Measures

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS)

The ADOS is a semi-structured play-based assessment of social interaction, communication, play skills, and restricted interests/repetitive behavior characteristic of ASD [65]. All participants received the Toddler Module, Module 1, or Module 2 of the ADOS or ADOS-2, administered by a certified examiner with a caregiver present in the room. The ADOS social affect calibrated severity score, which accounts for variation in raw scores that may be related to cognition [71, 72], was used for correlational analyses with SADOE. Higher scores indicate greater ASD-related behaviors.

Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile, Behavioral Sample (CSBS)

The CSBS behavioral sample is a semi-structured assessment collected in 24-month-old IBIS participants. The protocol consists of interactions between the examiner and child that are designed to elicit specific social and communicative behaviors. This measure is appropriate for children with a functional communication age between 6 to 24 months old [73]. Composite scores are calculated for Social, Speech, and Symbolic domains as well as a combined overall score. Composite social scores were used in correlational analyses with SADOE, with lower scores indicating greater impairment in social communicative behaviors.

Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL)

The MSEL is a developmental assessment battery that provides an index of general cognitive development in young children ages birth to 68 months old [74]. Its five subscales target skills in the areas of receptive language, expressive language, visual reception, fine motor, and gross motor. The Early Learning Composite score (ELC) reported here is the sum of four subscales, providing an overall developmental standard score. Lower scores indicate less advanced cognitive development.

Behavioral coding of social attention during object engagement (SADOE)

The behaviors selected for coding as part of the SADOE paradigm were based on an existing canine behavioral measure of human-directed social attentional bias, an aspect of increased canine sociability toward humans associated with domestication [62]. In this task-based measure, canines were presented with an opportunity to extract a sausage, a highly attractive food item, from a solvable puzzle box. Canines were allowed up to two minutes to attempt to open the box and retrieve the item while in the presence of a familiar human experimenter. To evaluate social attentional bias, the duration of the canines’ gaze toward the human experimenter and the puzzle box were coded. Using this task, domesticated dogs, who have been observed to engage in more prolonged contact with human caretakers than human-socialized wolves [75], were found to spend a greater proportion of time looking at the experimenter versus the box in comparison to wolves. We hypothesized that toddlers with ASD (or elevated familial ASD liability) who participated in an analogous play-based paradigm would display a lower proportion of time engaged in social looking than children without ASD, given characteristic symptoms involving reduced social orienting in ASD.

To translate the canine competing-stimulus paradigm to index naturalistic social attentional bias in toddlers, we coded portions of free play in the ADOS during which time the child was in the presence of their caregiver and an experimenter while interacting with a focal toy. The first three minutes of coded free play occurred at the start of the ADOS and entailed child-directed toy exploration in the absence of adult prompting. This was followed by integration of experimenter and caregiver bids toward the child while the child continued toy exploration. This sample thus allowed observation of the child’s behavior in the context of both self-directed behavior and social prompting, enhancing generalizability across a range of real-world contexts.

Three toys from the standard ADOS materials, a musical pop-up toy, musical piano toy, and animal sounds toy, were selected a priori as focal toys, since, like the puzzle box in the canine task, key sound and visual features of the toys were contingent on the subject’s actions. Further, based on the team’s extensive prior experience administering the ADOS, these toys were broadly appealing and sustained the attention of children with and without ASD. Coded instances of engagement with a focal toy (i.e., object engagement) were defined as at least 10 s of continuous interaction with the toy, beginning with physical contact and ending when the child moved away or engaged with a different object. During these periods, the child’s target of visual attention was coded as the focal toy (toy looking), the examiner or the caregiver (social looking), or none of these (other looking). Social looks were further categorized by their degree of spontaneity, here intended to reflect the initiative required from the child. Spontaneity was categorized based on the presence of potential prompting events for a social look in the preceding three seconds. Prompting events entailed actions by the examiner or caregiver such as touching the child or calling the child’s name (name/touch code), speech or movement directed toward the child (child-directed code), and speech or movement that was not specifically directed at the child (non-child-directed code). If none of these prompting events occurred in the three seconds before the look, the social look was considered spontaneous. In SADOE coding for Sample 1, social looks were coded as spontaneous or not spontaneous, as this sample provided initial testing for the SADOE measure. In Sample 2, the larger sample, non-spontaneous social looks were further specified as name/touch, child-directed, or non-child-directed.

The frequency and duration of each look type was calculated using the BORIS software package [76], which allows the user to flag a customized set of behaviors within a video. Participants were excluded if they interacted with the designated focal toys for less than one minute total or if their eyes were not consistently visible in the video recording. Raters were blinded to diagnosis and demonstrated high inter-rater reliability, established by co-coding an independent sample of ten videos (ICC = 0.93 for social looks, ICC > 0.84 across all look types; see Table 2). Indices of SADOE were calculated by dividing the number of each social look type by the total duration of focal toy engagement.

Table 2 Inter-rater reliability of video coding

Statistics

Indices obtained as part of coding SADOE were compared across groups of toddlers with and without ASD using independent samples t-tests performed in SPSS, with corrected p-values applied when group variances were significantly different based on Levene’s test for equality of variances. Effect sizes were calculated using Hedges’ g to account for Sample 1’s n < 20 participants. Primary analyses involved measurement of SADOE as the frequency of social looks during engagement with focal toys, while secondary analyses involved subcategories of social looks. Power calculations conducted using G-power indicated that at alpha = 0.05 with 80% power, both samples 1 and 2 were powered to detect large effect sizes (for Sample 1, effect size ≥ 1.5; for Sample 2, effect size ≥ 0.9). Spearman’s correlation was used to evaluate a hypothesized inverse relationship between SADOE and ordinal levels of ASD-related behaviors from ADOS social affect calibrated severity scores, while Pearson’s correlation was used to evaluate a hypothesized positive relationship between SADOE and continuous social scores on the CSBS. Binary logistic regression using pooled samples to maximize power tested whether 24-month SADOE was a concurrent predictor of ASD diagnosis. For all regression models, Hosmer-Lemershow tests were not significant, consistent with appropriate model fit. Variance was reported based on Nagelkerke’s pseudo-R2.

Results

Sample 1: comparison of SADOE in toddlers with and without ASD

Children with and without ASD were highly engaged with the focal toys, showing similar average durations of time in toy play (non-ASD = 3.5 min, ASD = 3.2 min; t(14) = 0.39, p = 0.70; Fig. 1A). While playing, children in both groups spent most of the time looking at the toy (non-ASD = 84%, ASD = 87%; t(14) = -0.95, p = 0.65; Fig. 1B) versus the experimenter, caregiver, or other aspects of the environment (Fig. 1B). However, the non-ASD group exhibited a significantly higher frequency of social looks than the ASD group (non-ASD = 3.3 looks per min, ASD = 1.3 looks per min; t(14) = 3.72, p = 0.002; Fig. 1C), indicating a reduced tendency for SADOE among toddlers with ASD. The effect size for this difference was large: Hedges’ g = 1.76, with 95% confidence intervals (CI) of 0.61 and 2.87. Similarly, the percentage of time spent on social looks differed between the groups, with the ASD-diagnosed toddlers spending less of their time looking at the experimenter and caregiver (non-ASD = 9% ASD = 2%; t(9) = 3.50, p = 0.006; Hedges’ g = 1.66; Fig. 1B).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Social Attention During Object Engagement (SADOE) is lower among children with versus without ASD. (A) Total time engaged with focal toys was not significantly different between groups (bars show mean ± 1 SE). (B) Proportions of time each group spent looking at social and nonsocial targets during engagement with the focal toys. (C) Non-ASD children made significantly more social looks per minute during engagement with the focal toys than children with ASD (p = .023; bars show mean ± 1 SE). non-ASD, without ASD; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; SE, standard error. N = 8 each for non-ASD and ASD groups

In terms of spontaneous looks based on the child’s self-directed initiative, toddlers without ASD made four times as many spontaneous social looks per minute than toddlers with ASD [non-ASD = 2.1 looks per min, ASD = 0.5 looks per min; t(8) = 2.77, p = 0.023; Hedges’ g = 1.31]. In contrast, the groups did not significantly differ in terms of social looks that were not spontaneous (non-ASD = 1.2 looks per min, ASD = 0.58 looks per min; t(14) = 1.01, p = 0.33). Overall, these results illustrate that toddlers with ASD are less attentive to social partners during object engagement than children without ASD.

Sample 2: comparison of SADOE in toddlers with differing familial ASD liability

Given the differences between toddlers with and without ASD in Sample 1, we next tested for differences among children categorized by level of family liability for ASD in the larger IBIS sample. Here, the LL- group had the lowest familial risk for ASD, followed by the HL- group, and lastly the HL + group, with high familial risk and an ASD diagnosis. By stratifying according to group-level familial ASD risk, we were able to investigate whether differences in SADOE were primarily associated with ASD diagnosis or ASD familial liability.

Like Sample 1, all groups demonstrated similar levels of engagement with focal toys (LL- = 4.2 min, HL- = 3.9 min, HL +  = 3.7 min; p’s > 0.47; Fig. 2A) and percentage of time looking at focal toys while engaged in play (LL- = 85%, HL- = 87%, HL +  = 90%; p’s ≥ 0.17; Fig. 2B). However, the HL + group with ASD showed the lowest frequency of social looks (Fig. 2C), similar to the ASD group in Sample 1. While the HL + group exhibited 1.2 social looks per minute of toy play, both the HL- group and the LL- group showed significantly more social looking, at 2.8 looks/min [t(44) = 3.25, p < 0.001] and 2.6 looks/min [t(29) = 2.90, p = 0.007], respectively. Effect sizes were large for both group differences, with confidence intervals that overlapped corresponding findings in Sample 1: LL- vs. HL + , Hedges’ g = 0.92 (95% CI: 0.21, 1.61) and HL- vs. HL + , Hedges’ g = 1.00 (95% CI: 0.36, 1.62). The HL + group also spent a lower percentage of time than the LL- and HL- groups on social looking while playing with the focal toys: (LL- = 6% time on social looks, HL- = 7% time on social looks, HL +  = 3% time on social looks). This comprised a significant difference between the HL + and HL- group [t(43) = 3.42, p = 0.001; Hedges’ g = 0.87] and a trend-level difference between the HL + and LL- group [t(29) = 1.67, p = 0.11; Hedges’ g = 0.56]. In contrast, neither the frequency of social looks nor the percentage of social looking time differed between the LL- and HL- groups (p’s > 0.75).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Social Attention During Object Engagement (SADOE) is greater among both high-likelihood (HL) and low-likelihood (LL) toddlers without ASD compared to toddlers with ASD. (A) Total time engaged with focal toys was not significantly different between groups (bars show mean ± 1 SE). B) Proportions of time each group spent looking at social and nonsocial targets during engagement with the focal toys. (C) HL- and LL- groups made more social looks per minute during play with the focal toys compared to the HL + group with ASD (LL- vs. HL + , p = .007; HL- vs. HL + , p < .001), with no significant difference in LL- versus HL- groups (p = .75; bars show mean ± 1 SE). Single asterisk indicates p < .01. Double asterisk indicates p < .001. (D) Number of social looks per minute by type for each group. Among social look types, the HL + group was lower than both the LL- and HL- groups specifically for spontaneous social looks (p’s ≤ .012). (E) Social look types as in (D) but expressed as a percentage of the total number of social looks. The HL + group displayed a lower percentage of spontaneous looks than the LL- and HL- groups (p’s ≤ .02). LL-, low-likelihood of ASD without a diagnosis (N = 19); HL-, high-likelihood of ASD without a diagnosis (N = 33); HL + , high-likelihood of ASD with a diagnosis (N = 15). ASD, autism spectrum disorder; SE, standard error

As found in Sample 1, the HL + group made significantly fewer spontaneous social looks per minute of toy play (Fig. 2D): 0.5 looks/min for the HL + group, with 1.4 looks/min for both the HL- [t(44) = 3.62, p < 0.001] and LL- groups [t(32) = 2.65, p = 0.012; Hedges’ g = 0.89 for both comparisons]. The LL- and HL- groups did not differ in spontaneous looks [t(32) = -0.2, p = 0.84]. Among subtypes of toddler social looking that were prompted by distinct caregiver or examiner behaviors (Fig. 2D, 2E), social looks following child-directed examiner/caregiver behaviors (with separate codes for “name/touch” and “child-directed” for other speech or movement) were significantly lower for the HL + versus the LL- group only [t(28) = 2.35, p = 0.026; Hedges’ g = 0.74]. Social looks following examiner/caregiver speech or movement not directed towards the child (coded as “non-child-directed”) were lower in the HL + versus HL- group only [t(41) = 3.44, p = 0.001; Hedges’ g = 0.75].

When considering the subtype of social looking as a percentage of all social looks (Fig. 2E), the HL + group also showed a lower percentage of spontaneous social looks compared to both LL- and HL- groups: LL-, [t(30) = 2.46, p = 0.02, Hedges’ g = 0.86]; HL-, [t(44) = 2.42, p = 0.02, Hedges’ g = 0.78]. No significant differences were found between the HL + group and LL- or HL- groups, or between LL- and HL- groups, for other subtypes of social looking. Collectively, these findings support the specificity of spontaneous social looks for differentiating toddlers with and without ASD, corroborating results from Sample 1, and indicate that diminished SADOE in toddlers is characteristic of ASD rather than familial liability for ASD.

Correlation of SADOE with measures of ASD-relevant social behaviors

We next investigated whether SADOE is related to existing social behavioral indices commonly used in assessment of children with ASD. SADOE was negatively correlated with ASD-related behaviors on the ADOS calibrated social affect severity score obtained from the same testing sessions across Samples 1 and 2, ρ = -0.50, p < 0.001 (Fig. 3A). SADOE was also positively correlated with the social composite score of the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales (CSBS), a measure of social skills available in Sample 2, r = 0.49, p < 0.001 (Fig. 3B). These complementary results suggest that SADOE is related but not identical to existing ASD-relevant measures of social behavior.

Fig.3
figure 3

Social Attention During Object Engagement (SADOE) correlates with other measures of social behavior. (A) SADOE was negatively correlated with the ADOS CSS score (ρ = -.50, p < .001). (B) SADOE was positively correlated with CSBS social score (r = .49, p = .001). ASD, autism spectrum disorder; non-ASD, without ASD; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; CSS, calibrated severity score; CSBS, Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales

SADOE and concurrent prediction of ASD diagnosis

Finally, binary logistic regression based on pooled data across both samples was performed to investigate the diagnostic predictive power of SADOE alone and in combination with the ELC, to assess contributions of cognitive development (Table 3). With SADOE as the only predictor, the model was significant (χ2(1) = 18.10, p < 0.001) and accounted for 29.4% of the variance in diagnosis while correctly classifying 74.1% of cases as ASD or non-ASD.

Table 3 Binary logistic regression for predictors of diagnostic status

When the ELC was added to the model, it remained significant (χ2(2) = 48.12, p < 0.001) and accounted for 65.7% of the variance in diagnosis, with 85.2% of cases being correctly classified by diagnostic status. Both SADOE and ELC were significant contributors to the model (Table 3), suggesting that SADOE provides useful diagnostic information independent of cognitive function for this sample.

Discussion

Reduced social orienting is a hallmark of ASD, and here we present the first human translation of a straightforward, cross-species method to quantify this phenomenon via a competing-stimulus approach in toddlers. By pitting a highly attractive nonsocial stimulus against the draw of social engagement during unstructured play, this assay enables sensitive quantification of naturalistic social orienting among toddlers with and without ASD. Consistent with a metric representing a core ASD feature, SADOE accounted for a substantial proportion of the variance in diagnosis, and the association between SADOE and diagnostic status remained significant when accounting for the impact of cognitive development. With an average of less than five minutes of play coded per child, this brief assay may be implemented without significant additional burden to families, clinicians, or researchers using the ADOS, a well-established diagnostic tool for ASD.

As predicted, children with ASD did not disengage their attention from the focal toys to look towards their caregiver and the experimenter as often as children without ASD. The effect size for this group difference was large in both samples, consistent with clinical impairment of social orienting in ASD. When toddlers with ASD did make social looks, they were also less likely to be spontaneous and more likely to be elicited by an adult’s behavior. SADOE also correlated in the expected direction with distinct measures of the level of ASD-related behaviors and social communication, in line with prior work showing strong interrelations between social characteristics contributing to ASD traits [27]. Nevertheless, its moderate correlation with ADOS social affect calibrated severity scores and CSBS social composite scores indicates that the SADOE metric captures uniquely informative social variation associated with ASD.

Contrary to our hypothesis regarding SADOE relative to familial liability for ASD, diminished SADOE in toddlers, whether considering all social looks or spontaneous social looks, appears to represent a marker of ASD itself rather than familial liability for ASD. SADOE scores did not differ between LL- and HL- toddlers and score distributions were highly overlapping. Given that decreased social orienting can arise before ASD diagnosis [12], future studies should investigate whether SADOE may stratify familial liability for subsequent ASD earlier in development and predict likelihood of ASD. Such developmentally sensitive differences have been postulated for measures of behaviors which, like social orienting, are heritable [25,26,27] and hypothesized to contribute to ASD’s ontogeny [77].

The present study provides compelling evidence that children with ASD display significantly less social orienting than their non-ASD peers in the context of a brief, naturalistic engagement with a high-interest nonsocial stimulus. Whether this reflects heightened object attention or diminished social attention remains uncertain; however, there was not a significant difference in the length of time that ASD versus non-ASD children spent engaged with the focal toys, nor in the time they spent looking at the toys during those engagements, suggesting that differences in social looks were not driven by excessive interest in the object. This similarity in attention to the focal toy, along with the strong contribution of spontaneous, self-initiated looks to ASD-associated differences in SADOE, supports the idea that SADOE may be more strongly influenced by social motivation to engage with others rather than object attraction. Of note, a previous study quantified social looking across the full ADOS administration and also found discrepancies in social attention between ASD and non-ASD children [78], consistent with the interpretation that our findings reflect impaired social motivation in ASD rather than simply greater object attraction.

Other measures such as the Early Social Communication Scale (ESCS) [79] present children with novel toys as a means to elicit joint attention and have similarly revealed that children with ASD are less likely to orient towards social partners in that context compared to neurotypically developing peers [80]. Comparable results have been demonstrated in terms of joint attention during naturalistic play [81]. While SADOE focuses on the more basic construct of social orienting, a critical precursor to joint attention, this literature aligns with our finding that children with ASD show reduced social looking during toy play and implies that impaired joint attention may be one downstream consequence of this failure to appropriately attend to social stimuli. Unlike the ESCS and other existing measures, SADOE has the practical advantage of being readily obtainable from video recordings of the widely used ADOS assessment and potentially other observational contexts, regardless of the child’s age or verbal ability. Follow-up studies could also evaluate clinical validity and reliability for a brief prospective SADOE assessment that could augment early ASD screening.

SADOE was intentionally designed to facilitate cross-species behavioral phenotyping by adapting a competing-stimulus paradigm previously established in canines [62] for use in human toddlers to measure an evolutionarily conserved aspect of social behavior implicated in ASD. Observations of reduced SADOE in toddlers with ASD are congruent, as hypothesized, with reduced task-based social orienting in wolves versus domesticated dogs, which unlike wolves, have an extensive co-evolutionary history with humans. This correspondence suggests that competing-stimulus approaches to social orienting designed to be comparable across species (e.g., by accounting for variations in cognitive abilities or socialization with humans versus conspecifics) could be informative in other animal models, such as rodents, with their flexibility to probe molecular genetic mechanisms, and non-human primates, with their high degree of genetic relatedness to humans. Additionally, the present finding, in the context of the association of canine social orienting with a conserved gene linked to human social behavior [62], as well as the known heritability of ASD [70] and human social orienting [25,26,27], supports strong translational opportunities in canine models. For example, application of SADOE in domestic dogs, which experience an early sensitive period for human socialization [82], could be used to investigate relationships between human exposure, social orienting, and DNA methylation profiles, including in the previously implicated oxytocin receptor gene (29, 57, 58). Such work could generate hypotheses about genes and epigenetic mechanisms playing a role in human social orienting and ASD, while elucidating biological pathways that inform targets for intervention. The feasibility of the competing-stimulus approach in human toddlers further underscores the versatility of canines as a human-analog model, considering the importance of toddlerhood for clarifying the early identification, ontogeny, and subsequent course of ASD symptomology.

Although the retrospective use of existing ADOS videos allowed us to capitalize on existing datasets, it imposed some limitations. While the semi-structured nature of the ADOS assessment moderated the range of possible responses from the experimenter and caregiver, there was still heterogeneity in the behavior of these adults, which could upwardly bias the total number of social looks by increasing children’s opportunity for non-spontaneous social looking. Relatedly, although the toy set was standardized, variables such as the precise placement of the toys and their location relative to social partners were not standardized across subjects. Subjects’ prior exposure to the high-interest toys was also not assessed, potentially adding the confound of familiarity versus novelty for quantifying social orienting relative to engagement with the focal toy. However, the difference in SADOE between ASD and non-ASD subjects was consistent enough to be observed to a similar degree in two independent samples, with differences driven by spontaneous social looks in both cases. Future directions include studies to validate this measure for clinical practice, as well as evaluating how more controlled settings might impact SADOE, as optimizing the balance of ecological validity with experimental control will be most informative for capturing variation in SADOE that differentiates children with and without ASD.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that a brief behavioral measure pitting a high-interest object against the innate draw of social engagement can serve as a rapid, feasible measure of social orienting in young children. By developing a human measure that is applicable to other species, this work opens the door to powerful approaches available in model organisms to explore underlying genetic and neurobiological influences on social orienting and how these factors may contribute to mechanisms of ASD and other neurodevelopmental disorders.

Availability of data and material

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ADOS:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

ASD:

Autism Spectrum Disorder

CSBS:

Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile, Behavior Sample

ESCS:

Early Social Communication Scale

ELC:

Early Learning Composite score of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning

HL-:

High-Likelihood Negative Group (infant siblings without an ASD diagnosis and an older sibling with ASD)

HL+:

High-Likelihood Positive Group (infant siblings with an ASD diagnosis and an older sibling with ASD)

LL-:

Low-Likelihood Negative Group (infant siblings without an ASD diagnosis or a first- or second-degree family history of ASD)

MSEL:

Mullen Scales of Early Learning

SADOE:

Social Attention During Object Engagement

References

  1. Valenza E, Simion F, Cassia VM, Umiltà C. Face preference at birth. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1996;22(4):892.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Rosa Salva O, Farroni T, Regolin L, Vallortigara G, Johnson MH. The evolution of social orienting: evidence from chicks (Gallus gallus) and human newborns. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(4):e18802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Farroni T, Csibra G, Simion F, Johnson MH. Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2002;99(14):9602–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Shultz S, Klin A, Jones W. Neonatal transitions in social behavior and their implications for autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018;22(5):452–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(4):231–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Salley B, Sheinkopf SJ, Neal-Beevers AR, Tenenbaum EJ, Miller-Loncar CL, Tronick E, et al. Infants’ early visual attention and social engagement as developmental precursors to joint attention. Dev Psychol. 2016;52(11):1721–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Ibanez LV, Grantz CJ, Messinger DS. The development of referential communication and autism symptomatology in high-risk infants. Infancy. 2013;18(5):687–707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Palomo R, Ozonoff S, Young GS, Belinchón Carmona M. Social orienting and initiated joint attention behaviors in 9 to 12 month old children with autism spectrum disorder: a family home movies study. Autism Res. 2022;15(6):1109–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Osterling J, Dawson G. Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24(3):247–57.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Osterling JA, Dawson G, Munson JA. Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14(2):239–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Maestro S, Muratori F, Cavallaro MC, Pei F, Stern D, Golse B, et al. Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(10):1239–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Jones W, Klin A. Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature. 2013;504(7480):427–31.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Papagiannopoulou EA, Chitty KM, Hermens DF, Hickie IB, Lagopoulos J. A systematic review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies in children with autism spectrum disorders. Soc Neurosci. 2014;9(6):610–32.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Frazier TW, Strauss M, Klingemier EW, Zetzer EE, Hardan AY, Eng C, et al. A meta-analysis of gaze differences to social and nonsocial information between individuals with and without autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56(7):546–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Klin A, Jones W, Schultz R, Volkmar F, Cohen D. Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(9):809–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Chawarska K, Macari S, Shic F. Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(8):903–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Pierce K, Marinero S, Hazin R, McKenna B, Barnes CC, Malige A. Eye tracking reveals abnormal visual preference for geometric images as an early biomarker of an autism spectrum disorder subtype associated with increased symptom severity. Biol Psychiat. 2016;79(8):657–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Sasson NJ, Touchstone EW. Visual attention to competing social and object images by preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(3):584–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Bacon EC, Moore A, Lee Q, Carter Barnes C, Courchesne E, Pierce K. Identifying prognostic markers in autism spectrum disorder using eye tracking. Autism. 2020;24(3):658–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Falck-Ytter T, Nyström P, Gredebäck G, Gliga T, Gliga T, Gliga T, et al. Reduced orienting to audiovisual synchrony in infancy predicts autism diagnosis at 3 years of age. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018;59(8):872–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Murias M, Major S, Davlantis K, Franz L, Harris A, Rardin B, et al. Validation of eye-tracking measures of social attention as a potential biomarker for autism clinical trials. Autism Res. 2018;11(1):166–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Campbell K, Carpenter KL, Hashemi J, Espinosa S, Marsan S, Borg JS, et al. Computer vision analysis captures atypical attention in toddlers with autism. Autism. 2019;23(3):619–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Bhat A, Galloway J, Landa R. Social and non-social visual attention patterns and associative learning in infants at risk for autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(9):989–97.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Unruh KE, Sasson NJ, Shafer RL, Whitten A, Miller SJ, Turner-Brown L, et al. Social orienting and attention is influenced by the presence of competing nonsocial information in adolescents with autism. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Constantino JN, Kennon-McGill S, Weichselbaum C, Marrus N, Haider A, Glowinski AL, et al. Infant viewing of social scenes is under genetic control and is atypical in autism. Nature. 2017;547(7663):340–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Kennedy DP, Kennedy DP, D’Onofrio BM, Quinn PD, Bölte S, Lichtenstein P, Falck-Ytter T. Genetic influence on eye movements to complex scenes at short timescales. Curr Biol. 2017;27(22):3554-60. e3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Marrus N, Grant JD, Harris-Olenak B, Albright J, Bolster D, Haber JR, et al. Genetic architecture of reciprocal social behavior in toddlers: Implications for heterogeneity in the early origins of autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2020;32(4):1190–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Wiśniowiecka-Kowalnik B, Nowakowska BA. Genetics and epigenetics of autism spectrum disorder: current evidence in the field. J Appl Genet. 2019;60(1):37–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Puglia MH, Connelly JJ, Morris JP. Epigenetic regulation of the oxytocin receptor is associated with neural response during selective social attention. Transl Psychiatry. 2018;8(1):1–10.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Bakulski K, Dou JF, Feinberg JI, Aung MT, Ladd-Acosta C, Volk HE, et al. Autism-associated DNA methylation at birth from multiple tissues is enriched for autism genes in the Early Autism Risk Longitudinal Investigation (EARLI). Front Mol Neurosci. 2021;14:775390.

  31. Hannon E, Schendel D, Ladd-Acosta C, Grove J, Hansen CS, Andrews SV, et al. Elevated polygenic burden for autism is associated with differential DNA methylation at birth. Genome Medicine. 2018;10(1):1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Marrus N, Faughn C, Shuman J, Petersen SE, Constantino JN, Povinelli DJ, et al. Initial description of a quantitative, cross-species (chimpanzee-human) social responsiveness measure. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(5):508–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Feczko EJ, Bliss-Moreau E, Walum H, Pruett JR Jr, Parr LA. The macaque social responsiveness scale (mSRS): a rapid screening tool for assessing variability in the social responsiveness of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). PLoS ONE. 2016;11(1):e0145956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Rosa-Salva O, Regolin L, Vallortigara G. Faces are special for newly hatched chicks: evidence for inborn domain-specific mechanisms underlying spontaneous preferences for face-like stimuli. Dev Sci. 2010;13(4):565–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Sugita Y. Face perception in monkeys reared with no exposure to faces. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2008;105(1):394–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Somppi S, Törnqvist H, Hänninen L, Krause C, Vainio O. Dogs do look at images: eye tracking in canine cognition research. Anim Cogn. 2012;15(2):163–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Kovács K, Kis A, Kanizsár O, Hernádi A, Gácsi M, Topál J. The effect of oxytocin on biological motion perception in dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cogn. 2016;19(3):513–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Brown J, Kaplan G, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. Perception of biological motion in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): by females only. Anim Cogn. 2010;13(3):555–64.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Vallortigara G, Regolin L, Marconato F. Visually inexperienced chicks exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patterns. PLoS Biol. 2005;3(7):e208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Paukner A, Simpson EA, Ferrari PF, Mrozek T, Suomi SJ. Neonatal imitation predicts how infants engage with faces. Dev Sci. 2014;17(6):833–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Machado CJ, Whitaker AM, Smith SE, Patterson PH, Bauman MD. Maternal immune activation in nonhuman primates alters social attention in juvenile offspring. Biol Psychiat. 2015;77(9):823–32.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Jastorff J, Popivanov ID, Vogels R, Vanduffel W, Orban GA. Integration of shape and motion cues in biological motion processing in the monkey STS. Neuroimage. 2012;60(2):911–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Topál J, Román V, Turcsán B. The dog (Canis familiaris) as a translational model of autism: it is high time we move from promise to reality. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2019;10(4):e1495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Hare B, Ferrans M. Is cognition the secret to working dog success? Anim Cogn. 2021;24(2):231–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Persson M, Roth L, Johnsson M, Wright D, Jensen P. Human-directed social behaviour in dogs shows significant heritability. Genes Brain Behav. 2015;14(4):337–44.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Bray EE, Gnanadesikan GE, Horschler DJ, Levy KM, Kennedy BS, Famula TR, et al. Early-emerging and highly heritable sensitivity to human communication in dogs. Curr Biol. 2021;31(14):3132-6. e5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Siniscalchi M, d’Ingeo S, Quaranta A. Orienting asymmetries and physiological reactivity in dogs’ response to human emotional faces. Learn Behav. 2018;46(4):574–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Johnston AM, Turrin C, Watson L, Arre AM, Santos LR. Uncovering the origins of dog-human eye contact: dingoes establish eye contact more than wolves, but less than dogs. Anim Behav. 2017;133:123–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Téglás E, Gergely A, Kupán K, Miklósi Á, Topál J. Dogs’ gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals. Curr Biol. 2012;22(3):209–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Worsley HK, O’Hara SJ. Cross-species referential signalling events in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cogn. 2018;21(4):457–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Topál J, Kis A, Oláh K. Dogs’ sensitivity to human ostensive cues: a unique adaptation? The Social Dog: Elsevier; 2014. p. 319–46.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Albuquerque N, Guo K, Wilkinson A, Savalli C, Otta E, Mills D. Dogs recognize dog and human emotions. Biol Let. 2016;12(1):20150883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Kano F, Shepherd SV, Hirata S, Call J. Primate social attention: species differences and effects of individual experience in humans, great apes, and macaques. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(2):e0193283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Hare B, Tomasello M. Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(9):439–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Karl S, Sladky R, Lamm C, Huber L. Neural responses of pet dogs witnessing their caregiver’s positive interactions with a conspecific an fMRI study. Cereb Cortex Commun. 2021;2(3):tgab047.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Nagasawa M, Mitsui S, En S, Ohtani N, Ohta M, Sakuma Y, et al. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science. 2015;348(6232):333–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Gregory SG, Connelly JJ, Towers AJ, Johnson J, Biscocho D, Markunas CA, et al. Genomic and epigenetic evidence for oxytocin receptor deficiency in autism. BMC Med. 2009;7(1):1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Cimarelli G, Virányi Z, Turcsán B, Rónai Z, Sasvári-Székely M, Bánlaki Z. Social behavior of pet dogs is associated with peripheral OXTR methylation. Front Psychol. 2017;8:549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Miklósi Á, Topal J, Csányi V. Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us? Anim Behav. 2004;67(6):995–1004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Galambos Á, Petró E, Nagy B, Turcsán B, Topál J. The effects of social and non-social distracting stimuli on dogs with different levels of social competence: empirical evidence for a canine model of autism. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2021;244:105451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Gácsi M, Virányi Z, Csányi V. A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Curr Biol. 2003;13(9):763–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. VonHoldt BM, Shuldiner E, Koch IJ, Kartzinel RY, Hogan A, Brubaker L, et al. Structural variants in genes associated with human Williams-Beuren syndrome underlie stereotypical hypersociability in domestic dogs. Sci Adv. 2017;3(7):e1700398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Jakovcevic A, Mustaca A, Bentosela M. Do more sociable dogs gaze longer to the human face than less sociable ones? Behav Proc. 2012;90(2):217–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Horn L, Virányi Z, Miklósi A, Huber L, Range F. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) flexibly adjust their human-directed behavior to the actions of their human partners in a problem situation. Anim Cogn. 2012;15(1):57–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule (2nd ed.): Modules 1–4. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2012.

  66. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(3):205–23.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. Marrus N, Glowinski AL, Jacob T, Klin A, Jones W, Drain CE, et al. Rapid video-referenced ratings of reciprocal social behavior in toddlers: a twin study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(12):1338–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Bradshaw J, Gillespie S, Klaiman C, Klin A, Saulnier C. Early emergence of discrepancy in adaptive behavior and cognitive skills in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2019;23(6):1485–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Estes A, Zwaigenbaum L, Gu H, St John T, Paterson S, Elison JT, et al. Behavioral, cognitive, and adaptive development in infants with autism spectrum disorder in the first 2 years of life. J Neurodev Disord. 2015;7(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Sandin S, Lichtenstein P, Kuja-Halkola R, Hultman C, Larsson H, Reichenberg A. The heritability of autism spectrum disorder. JAMA. 2017;318(12):1182–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Esler AN, Bal VH, Guthrie W, Wetherby A, Weismer SE, Lord C. The autism diagnostic observation schedule, toddler module: standardized severity scores. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(9):2704–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Hus V, Gotham K, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS domain scores: Separating severity of social affect and restricted and repetitive behaviors. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(10):2400–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Wetherby AM, Allen L, Cleary J, Kublin K, Goldstein H. Validity and reliability of the communication and symbolic behavior scales developmental profile with very young children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(6):1202–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Mullen E. Mullen: Scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guideline Service; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Lazzaroni M, Range F, Backes J, Portele K, Scheck K, Marshall-Pescini S. The effect of domestication and experience on the social interaction of dogs and wolves with a human companion. Front Psychol. 2020;11:785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Friard O, Gamba M. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol Evol. 2016;7(11):1325–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Elison JT. Considering transient instantiators. Dev Psychopathol. 2020;32(4):1173–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Mosconi MW, Steven Reznick J, Mesibov G, Piven J. The social orienting continuum and response scale (SOC-RS): a dimensional measure for preschool-aged children. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(2):242–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Mundy P, Delgado C, Block J, Venezia M, Hogan A, Seibert J. Early social communication scales (ESCS). Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Mundy P, Sigman M, Kasari C. Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism. Dev Psychopathol. 1994;6(3):389–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Roos EM, McDuffie AS, Weismer SE, Gernsbacher MA. A comparison of contexts for assessing joint attention in toddlers on the autism spectrum. Autism. 2008;12(3):275–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Scott JP, Fuller JL. Genetics and the social behavior of the dog. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Anna Abbacchi for consultation on the ADOS assessment and Yi Zhang for assistance with data management. We thank the families for their time and participation.

Funding

This study was supported by National Institutes of Health Autism Center of Excellence R01 grant (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, HD055741 to KB as subcontract site PI for PI Joseph Piven); Autism Speaks 6020 to KB as subcontract site PI for PI Joseph Piven; the Simons Foundation 140209 to KB as subcontract site PI for PI Joseph Piven; the National Institute of Mental Health (K08 MH112891 to NM and R01 MH100027 to JNC); National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01 HD068479 to JNC), and the Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Centers at Washington University (National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Child Health and Human Development 1P50 HD103525 to JNC).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CW, NM, and JNC conceptualized and designed the study. CW developed the video coding scheme, coded videos, performed primary data analyses, and drafted an initial version of the manuscript. NM and NH revised the manuscript and updated analyses. NH and JA coded videos and contributed to coding scheme revisions. KB contributed to data acquisition and curation. JD, NM, and JNC supervised the research and contributed to manuscript writing. All authors reviewed the manuscript for scientific content and interpretation, provided feedback, and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Natasha Marrus.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Infant Brain Imaging Study is approved by the Washington University School of Medicine IRB (#201103140, #201301110, #201105377, and #201208010). Informed consent was obtained from the parents of participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

JNC receives royalties from Western Psychological Services for the Social Responsiveness Scale. The other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Weichselbaum, C., Hendrix, N., Albright, J. et al. Social attention during object engagement: toward a cross-species measure of preferential social orienting. J Neurodevelop Disord 14, 58 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-022-09467-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-022-09467-5

Keywords