- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Associations of HLA alleles with specific language impairment
Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders volume 6, Article number: 1 (2014)
Abstract
Background
Human leukocyte antigen (HLA) loci have been implicated in several neurodevelopmental disorders in which language is affected. However, to date, no studies have investigated the possible involvement of HLA loci in specific language impairment (SLI), a disorder that is defined primarily upon unexpected language impairment. We report association analyses of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and HLA types in a cohort of individuals affected by language impairment.
Methods
We perform quantitative association analyses of three linguistic measures and case-control association analyses using both SNP data and imputed HLA types.
Results
Quantitative association analyses of imputed HLA types suggested a role for the HLA-A locus in susceptibility to SLI. HLA-A A1 was associated with a measure of short-term memory (P = 0.004) and A3 with expressive language ability (P = 0.006). Parent-of-origin effects were found between HLA-B B8 and HLA-DQA1*0501 and receptive language. These alleles have a negative correlation with receptive language ability when inherited from the mother (P = 0.021, P = 0.034, respectively) but are positively correlated with the same trait when paternally inherited (P = 0.013, P = 0.029, respectively). Finally, case control analyses using imputed HLA types indicated that the DR10 allele of HLA-DRB1 was more frequent in individuals with SLI than population controls (P = 0.004, relative risk = 2.575), as has been reported for individuals with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Conclusion
These preliminary data provide an intriguing link to those described by previous studies of other neurodevelopmental disorders and suggest a possible role for HLA loci in language disorders.
Background
The human major histocompatibility complex on chromosome 6 contains many highly polymorphic genes involved in immune function [1, 2], notably the human leukocyte antigen (HLA) genes. Specific HLA alleles have been associated with more than 100 traits, including striking association with autoimmune diseases [3, 4]. Given the identification of possible abnormal immune responses observed in participants with autism (for example, antibodies which react to proteins found in the central nervous system [5]) and similarities between schizophrenia and diseases with established HLA associations [6], several studies have examined the possible involvement of HLA loci in neurodevelopmental disorders. Such investigations have typically targeted autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as well as psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia. Specific alleles (please see a note regarding the terminology in the Methods section) of HLA-DRB1, including DR4 and DR12, have been associated with autism, ADHD, and schizophrenia, although these findings have not always replicated within or between disorders. In addition, association between the A2 allele of the HLA-A gene and autism has been reported [7].
Pertinent to the present study is the suggestion that autoimmunity may also be involved in neurodevelopmental disorders that include language deficits: a positive autoimmune reaction to myelin was detected in children with Landau-Kleffner syndrome, a disorder characterized by aphasia and epileptic seizures [8]. Autoantibodies that react with brain tissue have been found in a relatively large number of children with an atypical variant of Landau-Kleffner syndrome and autism compared with controls. Such antibodies were also found in children affected by other neurological disorders, albeit less frequently [9]. Another study found antibodies that bound to rodent Purkinje cells in the serum of a mother with a child with specific language disorder, a child with autism, and a child with typical development. When this serum was injected in pregnant mice, the mouse pups had reduced exploratory behavior and impaired motor coordination [10]. The HLA region has also been implicated in a linkage study of dyslexia [11].
Specific language impairment (SLI) is diagnosed when a child has problems with the acquisition of language but shows normal development in all other areas [12]. SLI is a heterogeneous and complex disorder with a strong genetic component [13]. Several linkage regions and genes have been identified in genetic studies of SLI [14–17]. One gene in particular, CNTNAP2 (contactin associated protein-like 2), has been implicated in SLI, autism, and ADHD [16, 18, 19]; in the context of SLI, this gene was associated across three language-related measures: nonword repetition (NWR) scores, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF) expressive language scores (ELS), and CELF receptive language scores (RLS) [16], all of which have been employed in the present study. The product of the CNTNAP2 gene is involved in forming the gaps in the myelin sheath [20], which is interesting in the context of the autoimmune reaction to myelin detected in children with Landau-Kleffner syndrome. Though clinically distinct, SLI, autism, and ADHD have some phenotypic similarities; language may be impaired in all three disorders [21–23]. The core linguistic deficit in SLI may not be the same as the one in autism or ADHD. Children with ADHD and children with SLI may differ in their conversational profiles. Whereas children with SLI tend to have more limitations in the areas of lexical diversity, sentence length, and morphosyntactic development, children with ADHD differ from children with normal language in their utterance formulation (that is, in the number of pauses, repetitions, revisions, or other such elements), and children with autism typically have pragmatic impairments [24–26]. Nevertheless, some children with autism exhibit language impairments that are more frequently observed in children with SLI and vice versa[22]. Given the genetic and phenotypic overlaps between SLI, autism, and ADHD and the HLA linkage and associations identified in studies of dyslexia, autism, and ADHD, we decided to investigate the possible involvement of HLA loci in SLI. Our initial investigations involved the completion of quantitative and case-control association analyses with single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) across the HLA region. We went on to impute HLA types from SNP array data and tested for association with language impairment, again within quantitative-trait and case-control association models.
Methods
Participants
The SLI cohort in this study included families from five centers across the UK—the Newcomen Centre at Guy’s Hospital (London) (now called the Evelina Children’s Hospital), the Cambridge Language and Speech Project (CLASP), the Child Life and Health Department at the University of Edinburgh, the Department of Child Health at the University of Aberdeen, and the Manchester Language Study—as described in previous SLI Consortium (SLIC) studies [14, 17, 27–29]. In addition, this study included 49 newly-collected families from the Evelina Children’s Hospital cohort which had not been included in previous SLIC studies. All individuals were self-reported white British. These collections were approved by the relevant local ethics committees, and all subjects provided informed consent for participation in this study. All SLIC families were ascertained on the basis of a single proband with expressive and/or receptive language skills at least 1.5 standard deviations (SDs) below that expected for their age (population mean = 100, SD = 15) and Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) Perceptual Organization Index (a composite score of the non-verbal subtests Picture Completion, Picture Arrangement, Block Design, and Object Assembly) of more than 77.5 (1.5 SD below that expected for their age). Probands did not have a diagnosis of autism or hearing impairment. DNA and phenotypic data were collected from all children regardless of linguistic ability, and DNA was collected from all available parents. All probands met the above criteria for SLI, but we did not exclude probands on the basis of a diagnosis of ADHD or dyslexia alone, given the high degree of co-occurrence of SLI and ADHD or dyslexia. However, for some of our SLIC samples, data were available for the presence of hyperactivity, coordination, and reading problems. From this, we estimate that approximately one third of our SLIC samples showed some evidence of ADHD or developmental coordination disorder and that approximately one half of our probands had reading problems.
SNP arrays and quality control measures
SNP data were generated for both SLIC and control samples by using Illumina Omni-Express (version 12.1) genome-wide SNP arrays (Illumina, San Diego, CA, USA). Prior to HLA imputation, all genotypes were filtered on the basis of several quality-control measures as suggested in [30]: samples with a genotype success rate below 95% or a heterozygosity rate ±2 SD from the mean were removed, as were SNPs with a minor allele frequency of less than 1%, or a Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium P value of less than 0.001. Inheritance data within families were used to exclude SNPs and samples with an error rate of greater than 1%. Control data (CEU, YRI, CHB, JPT, Hapmap release #3) were employed through a principal component analysis to exclude individuals with divergent ancestry (although some minor differences due to population substructure may still be present), and samples with known chromosomal rearrangements or discordant sex information (where appropriate) were removed. Genotypes were compared with existing SNP genotype data for 700 individuals across 734 SNPs, yielding an estimated genotype error rate of 0.00967.
Imputation and grouping of HLA types
HLA alleles for HLA-A, HLA-B, HLA-C, HLA-DQA1, HLA-DQB1, and HLA-DRB1 were imputed from SNP data across chromosome 6 with HLA*IMP:01 [31]. HLA*IMP:01 employed 208 SNPs from across chromosome 6 which it deems informative to impute HLA types for the six HLA loci. The HLA*IMP algorithm first selects informative SNPs that were typed in both the reference and the sample sets. The client program performs several quality-control measures on the SNP data. Genotypes are aligned and phased within HLA*IMP by using a reference panel (HapMap3 CEU b36). The HLA types are then imputed on the HLA*IMP server.
HLA*IMP provides a probability value Q (maximum of the posterior probability distribution, which includes all the probabilities assigned to each allele of a given HLA gene) that may be used as a quality measure. We kept imputations with a Q score of 0.9 or above. The resolution of the HLA*IMP imputations is a four-digit HLA type. To reduce the number of alleles in the analyses while still retaining antigen-specificity distinctions, we grouped the HLA types according to the HLA Dictionary, based on their serological equivalence [32], as previously employed in similar studies [33]. For simplicity, the serological groups will hereafter be referred to as alleles. HLA-DQA1 types were not grouped, since HLA-DQA1 types were not included in the HLA Dictionary, and they are presented with their four-digit designations (for example, HLA-DQA1*0501). Alleles that otherwise did not have a WHO assignment were not used in this study.
A comparison of the imputations for a subset of the People of the British Isles (POBI) individuals who were typed for several HLA loci through direct genotyping (samples included in [34]) showed a concordance rate of 91.2% between the genotypes and imputations with Q ≥ 0.9 (122 alleles across all six loci were used in this comparison). It should be noted that the samples were genotyped and not serotyped, and a two-digit resolution for the HLA types was used.
Quantitative analyses
Three linguistic test scores were investigated by quantitative association analyses. Age-normalized composite scores of expressive and receptive language ability were obtained from the revised version of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-R) [35]. These scores provide a broad overview of two main components of linguistic ability and have previously been shown to represent consistent phenotypes for quantitative linkage and association analyses of SLI [14, 16]. A 28-item NWR test [36] was also administered. NWR may be used as a measure of phonological short-term memory (the ability to retain verbal or speech-based information for a short period of time), and it has also been proposed to be an endophenotype of SLI [37], although its sensitivity is disputed by some [38]. These measures were collected for all SLIC children regardless of language ability. Table 1 contains the descriptive statistics for the language traits of the sample used in this study, and the distribution plots can be found in Additional file 1. For an extensive description of the cohort statistics, see [14, 17, 27, 29]. Age-normalized scores were used for all samples, regardless of language ability. Nine hundred fourteen individuals from 280 language-impaired families were included in the quantitative analyses of both SNP data and HLA types. SNP association analyses were performed using a likelihood-ratio test (“assoc” function) in MERLIN [39]. The analyses included 6,725 SNPs encompassing the HLA region on chromosome 6 (25-35 MB, hg18). Since the imputed HLA types were multi-allelic, quantitative transmission disequilibrium test (QTDT) [40] was used for the family-based quantitative association analyses of these data. The orthogonal “within family” model was used, and the tests considered environmental, polygenic, and additive major gene effect variance components. Three tests were run: a general QTDT and two tests of parent-of-origin effects (maternal and paternal). We also tested for significant differences between the maternal and paternal associations in QTDT.
Case-control analyses
In addition to quantitative tests of association, we performed case-control analyses to compare HLA allele frequencies between independent probands and population controls. These analyses included 241 independent probands and 566 population controls. To control for non-independence, case-control analyses used only the proband from each family, even if the sibling met criteria for SLI (as described in the Participants section). The population controls came from the POBI study [34] and an Oxfordshire study of gene expression in primary immune cells [41] and were included in the principal component analysis mentioned earlier. All control samples were from adults who were unselected in terms of their language performance, and we had no phenotype data for them. SNP-based case-control analyses were performed using the standard case-control analysis based on a chi-squared test (“assoc” function) in PLINK [42]. SNP-based analyses included 6,725 SNPs encompassing the HLA region on chromosome 6 (25-35 MB, hg18). Since the imputed HLA-types were multi-allelic, we used a Fisher’s exact test calculator [43] to generate P values for differences in allele counts between cases and controls.
Calculation of relative risks for HLA data
Relative risks were calculated for HLA alleles that were significantly associated with increased or decreased risk in the case-control analysis. Samples with one or more unknown alleles (at a given locus) were excluded from these analyses. The calculations of relative risks and confidence intervals were performed by using the method described in [44].
Multiple testing of SNP and HLA data
In the quantitative analyses of HLA types, empirical P values were obtained for each of the tests performed with a permutation procedure implemented in QTDT using 1,000 permutations. QTDT performs Monte-Carlo permutations providing a P value with a built-in adjustment for multiple testing which is specific to the dataset in hand. Associations with empirical P values below 0.05 are marked by a superscript ‘a’ in Table 2. This method was chosen, as QTDT may test different numbers of alleles in each analysis, depending on the availability of informative families, which is taken into account through the built-in adjustments. In the case-control analysis of HLA-types as well as all the analyses which used the SNP data, the false discovery rate (FDR) method [45] was used to control for the proportion of false positives among the significant results. QVALUE, which uses the model proposed by Storey [46], was used for generating FDR q values for the alleles of each HLA locus separately (with λ = 0). The q values for SNP associations and HLA allele associations with P values below 0.05 are reported in the Results section below. This approach is particularly suitable for analyzing a large number of alleles, as using a Bonferroni correction in such cases would be highly over-conservative, resulting in a great reduction in power [47].
Results
Quantitative association analysis with SNP data
The most associated SNPs in the analyses of the ELS and RLS traits (rs1319972, P = 7.72 × 10−5 and rs1233579, P = 0.00026, respectively) did not fall within coding genes. The most associated SNP in the NWR analysis (rs17266491) fell within the gene LRRC16A (leucine rich repeat containing 16A) (P = 6.86 × 10−5) and represented the most significant result of the SNP-based quantitative analyses. Across the six HLA genes (HLA-A, HLA-B, HLA-C, HLA-DQA1, HLA-DQB1, and HLA-DRB1), the most associated SNP was rs3823342 in HLA-A. This was found with ELS (P = 0.01173) and had a q value of 0.205. Investigation of multiple testing using the FDR method yielded minimum q values of 0.057, 0.064, and 0.155 for the ELS, RLS, and NWR traits, respectively. These data indicate that between 5.7% and 15.5% of the highest-ranking SNP-based associations, of which there were 40 for ELS, 46 for RLS, and 6 for NWR, are likely to represent false positives. Regional association plots for the quantitative analyses can be found in Additional file 1.
Case-control association analysis with SNP data
The SNP with the strongest evidence for association (rs9467476, with P = 0.00027) fell within the gene LRRC16A. There were five other SNPs within that gene with P ≤0.01, but no other association trends were found. This association did not survive multiple testing: the lowest q value was 0.671, indicating a particularly high false-positive rate in these analyses. Across the six HLA genes examined, the most highly associated SNP was rs1058026 in HLA-B (P = 0.001885). A regional association plot for the case-control analysis can be found in Additional file 1.
Quantitative association analyses with HLA data
In total, nine alleles in HLA-A, HLA-B, HLA-C, HLA-DQA1, and HLA-DRB1 showed marginal association (nominal P ≤0.05). Furthermore, 8 of these 13 associations survived multiple testing, with an empirical P ≤0.05, as assessed by a permutation procedure. The most significant association was seen with allele A1 of the HLA-A gene, which was associated with increased performance on the NWR test (nominal P = 0.004, empirical P = 0.017). In contrast, the A3 allele of HLA-A was associated with lower ELSs (nominal P = 0.006, empirical P = 0.016). A marginal association was also found between the Cw8 allele of HLA-C, and higher ELSs (nominal P = 0.039, empirical P = 0.048).
The parent-of-origin analyses highlighted effects of particular interest for the B8 allele of the HLA-B gene (minimum nominal P = 0.013, empirical P = 0.029) and HLA-DQA1*0501 (minimum nominal P = 0.029, empirical P = 0.057). Both these alleles were associated with lower RLS when inherited from the mother and higher RLS when inherited from the father. Note, however, that the paternal parent-of-origin effects for HLA-DQA1*0501 did not survive the permutation procedure. Borderline associations were also observed for the paternal alleles Cw6 of HLA-C with ELS (nominal P = 0.019, empirical P = 0.037) and HLA-DQA1*0301 with RLS (nominal P = 0.011, empirical P = 0.035), which were associated with lower language scores. Table 2 includes all quantitative associations with P ≤0.05.
Case-control association analyses with HLA data
In total, there were three HLA-DRB1 alleles (DR4, DR10, and DR12) with nominal P ≤0.05. The DR10 allele of HLA-DRB1 showed a significantly increased allele frequency in probands with language impairment compared with controls (nominal P = 0.004), whereas the DR4 and DR12 alleles of HLA-DRB1 (nominal P = 0.02 and nominal P = 0.05, respectively) were less frequent in probands with language-impairment than controls indicating a protective risk. FDR analyses indicated that two of these three associations are likely to represent true positives (the q value for the least associated allele was 0.235). The DR10 allele had a particularly strong relative risk of 2.575 (95% confidence interval: 1.773 ≤ relative risk ≤ 3.737), whereas those for DR4 and DR12 were both below 1.0 and the 95% confidence intervals for DR4 did not encompass 1.0. HLA-DQA1*0301 (nominal P = 0.014) and the B57 allele of HLA-B (nominal P = 0.029) were also both significantly less frequent in probands with language-impairment compared with controls. FDR analyses of DQA1*0301 yielded a low q value (0.098), bolstering confidence in its authenticity. In contrast, FDR indicated that the association with HLA-B B57 is likely to represent a false positive (q value of 0.911). The relative risk values for DQA1*0301 and B57 were both below 1.0 and had significant 95% confidence intervals that did not encompass 1.0. Table 3 includes case-control associations with P ≤0.05 and their relative risks and confidence intervals. Figure 1 includes allele frequencies in probands with language impairment and controls for HLA loci tested.
Discussion
SLI is a complex and heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorder that affects language development in children. HLA associations have been reported for other neurodevelopmental disorders that show some genetic and phenotypic overlap with SLI. We investigated the possible involvement of HLA loci in SLI using several approaches: quantitative and case-control association analyses both with SNPs in and around the HLA region and with HLA types.
QTDT analyses of HLA-type data identified risk and protective alleles with regard to the three measured traits, and these findings did survive multiple testing. HLA-A A1 was the most highly positively correlated allele (with NWR, nominal P = 0.004, empirical P = 0.017), and HLA-A A3 was the most highly negatively correlated allele (with ELS, nominal P = 0.006, empirical P = 0.016), thus implicating the HLA-A locus in susceptibility to SLI.
We also found interesting evidence of parent-of-origin effects. In particular, the HLA-B B8 allele and HLA-DQA1*0501 are negatively correlated with RLS when inherited from the mother but positively correlated with the same trait when paternally inherited. Parent-of-origin effects have been reported with regard to the involvement of HLA alleles in neurological disorders such as multiple sclerosis [48], and in this respect it is interesting to find such an effect in SLI. The associations with B8 in both parent-of-origin effects analyses remained significant following the permutation procedure. Interestingly, HLA-B has been implicated in schizophrenia; however, the increase in risk was the result of matching HLA-B genotypes in the mother and the child [33]. While the association detected in our study is with a specific allele, it is interesting to note that the association is correlated with negative test scores only when the allele is inherited from the mother (so there is a matching of at least one allele between the mother and the child).
Although many of the associations observed in this study were of borderline significance, it is interesting to note that the identified association trends are supported by studies of related neurodevelopmental disorders. In an ADHD study of a Chinese cohort [49], the DR10 allele of HLA-DRB1 was found to be significantly more frequent in children with ADHD than in controls, as is the case with the probands with SLI in our study. This result translates to a particularly high relative risk of 2.575, with a 95% confidence interval of 1.773 ≤ relative risk ≤ 3.737, and was supported by FDR analyses. Similarly, Wang and colleagues [49] reported that the DR12 allele of HLA-DRB1 was significantly more frequent in controls than in cases with ADHD, a trend which again is replicated in our study. The trend of association found between the DR4 allele of HLA-DRB1 in our probands with language-impairment matches that described by Wright and colleagues [50] in a study of schizophrenia but is the opposite of that observed in ADHD by Odell and colleagues [51]. Note, however, that this ADHD association was not replicated in the Chinese study previously mentioned [49]. Interestingly, the HLA-DRB1 case-control associations observed in the present study also seem to be the opposite to previously described association trends of HLA alleles with autism (albeit not always significantly so). The DR4 allele was associated with autism [52, 53] but is significantly less frequent in probands with SLI than in controls (Table 3, Figure 1). DR13 and DR14 (grouped together) have been negatively associated with autism [52], which is, again, the opposite trend to the one observed here (Figure 1, frequency in probands with language-impairment: 0.14 and 0.03; frequency in controls: 0.11 and 0.02, for DR13 and DR14, respectively). Although these differences were not even nominally significant in SLI, they may suggest that if HLA alleles play a role in both SLI and autism, the mechanism itself may be different.
Although quantitative and case-control SNP-based analyses consistently identified associations within the LRRC16A gene, none of the SNP-based analyses found strong association trends within HLA loci. Furthermore, the FDR q values obtained for SNP-based associations were all greater than 0.05, indicating a high false-positive rate. Nonetheless, it should be noted that one might not expect to find a direct correlation between SNPs in HLA genes and effects mediated by HLA types, because of the high degree of variation in the HLA region.
Conclusions
The results of this study suggest a potential involvement of HLA loci in SLI. Quantitative association analyses highlighted HLA-A and parent-of-origin effects for HLA-B, whereas case-control analyses implicated HLA-DRB1 alleles. Further, larger-scale studies will be required to replicate these findings. The relatively small sample sizes employed are reflected by nominal P values. Since our analyses used imputed HLA alleles and since no HLA associations have been previously reported for SLI, we did not confine ourselves to testing only a subset of the HLA alleles, and, consequently, we performed a relatively large number of tests. Nonetheless, we believe that association testing and imputation of HLA types provide a cost-effective way of studying the involvement of immune-related genes in neurodevelopmental disorders. The preliminary data presented here provide an intriguing link to those described by previous studies of other neurodevelopmental disorders suggesting a possible role for HLA loci in language disorders.
Abbreviations
- ADHD:
-
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
- ELS:
-
Expressive language score
- FDR:
-
False discovery rate
- HLA:
-
Human leukocyte antigen
- NWR:
-
Nonword repetition (score)
- POBI:
-
People of the British Isles
- RLS:
-
Receptive language score
- SD:
-
Standard deviation
- SLI:
-
Specific language impairment
- SNP:
-
Single-nucleotide polymorphism.
References
Thomson G: A review of theoretical aspects of HLA and disease associations. Theor Popul Biol. 1981, 20: 168-208. 10.1016/0040-5809(81)90009-5.
Vandiedonck C, Knight JC: The human Major Histocompatibility Complex as a paradigm in genomics research. Brief Funct Genomic Proteomic. 2009, 8: 379-394. 10.1093/bfgp/elp010.
Shiina T, Inoko H, Kulski JK: An update of the HLA genomic region, locus information and disease associations: 2004. Tissue Antigens. 2004, 64: 631-649. 10.1111/j.1399-0039.2004.00327.x.
Fernando MM, Stevens CR, Walsh EC, De Jager PL, Goyette P, Plenge RM, Vyse TJ, Rioux JD: Defining the role of the MHC in autoimmunity: a review and pooled analysis. PLoS Genet. 2008, 4: e1000024-10.1371/journal.pgen.1000024.
Ashwood P, Van de Water J: A review of autism and the immune response. Clin Dev Immunol. 2004, 11: 165-174. 10.1080/10446670410001722096.
Wright P, Nimgaonkar VL, Donaldson PT, Murray RM: Schizophrenia and HLA: a review. Schizophr Res. 2001, 47: 1-12. 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00022-0.
Torres AR, Sweeten TL, Cutler A, Bedke BJ, Fillmore M, Stubbs EG, Odell D: The association and linkage of the HLA-A2 class I allele with autism. Hum Immunol. 2006, 67: 346-351. 10.1016/j.humimm.2006.01.001.
Nevsimalova S, Tauberova A, Doutlik S, Kucera V, Dlouha O: A role of autoimmunity in the etiopathogenesis of Landau-Kleffner syndrome?. Brain Dev. 1992, 14: 342-345. 10.1016/S0387-7604(12)80157-4.
Connolly AM, Chez MG, Pestronk A, Arnold ST, Mehta S, Deuel RK: Serum autoantibodies to brain in Landau-Kleffner variant, autism, and other neurologic disorders. J Pediatr. 1999, 134: 607-613. 10.1016/S0022-3476(99)70248-9.
Dalton P, Deacon R, Blamire A, Pike M, McKinlay I, Stein J, Styles P, Vincent A: Maternal neuronal antibodies associated with autism and a language disorder. Ann Neurol. 2003, 53: 533-537. 10.1002/ana.10557.
Cardon LR, Smith SD, Fulker DW, Kimberling WJ, Pennington BF, DeFries JC: Quantitative trait locus for reading disability on chromosome 6. Science. 1994, 266: 276-279. 10.1126/science.7939663.
Bishop DVM: What causes specific language impairment in children?. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2006, 15: 217-221. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00439.x.
Newbury DF, Bishop DV, Monaco AP: Genetic influences on language impairment and phonological short-term memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005, 9: 528-534. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.002.
The SLI Consortium: A genomewide scan identifies two novel loci involved in specific language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2002, 70: 384-398. 10.1086/338649.
Bartlett CW, Flax JF, Logue MW, Vieland VJ, Bassett AS, Tallal P, Brzustowicz LM: A major susceptibility locus for specific language impairment is located on 13q21. Am J Hum Genet. 2002, 71: 45-55. 10.1086/341095.
Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, Alarcón M, Oliver PL, Davies KE, Geschwind DH, Monaco AP, Fisher SE: A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. N Engl J Med. 2008, 359: 2337-2345. 10.1056/NEJMoa0802828.
Newbury DF, Winchester L, Addis L, Paracchini S, Buckingham LL, Clark A, Cohen W, Cowie H, Dworzynski K, Everitt A, Goodyer IM, Hennessy E, Kindley AD, Miller LL, Nasir J, O’Hare A, Shaw D, Simkin Z, Simonoff E, Slonims V, Watson J, Ragoussis J, Fisher SE, Seckl JR, Helms PJ, Bolton PF, Pickles A, Conti-Ramsden G, Baird G, Bishop DV, Monaco AP: CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2009, 85: 264-272. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.07.004.
Alarcón M, Abrahams BS, Stone JL, Duvall JA, Perederiy JV, Bomar JM, Sebat J, Wigler M, Martin CL, Ledbetter DH, Nelson SF, Cantor RM, Geschwind DH: Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. Am J Hum Genet. 2008, 82: 150-159. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.005.
Elia J, Gai X, Xie HM, Perin JC, Geiger E, Glessner JT, D’arcy M, deBerardinis R, Frackelton E, Kim C, Lantieri F, Muganga BM, Wang L, Takeda T, Rappaport EF, Grant SF, Berrettini W, Devoto M, Shaikh TH, Hakonarson H, White PS: Rare structural variants found in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental genes. Mol Psychiatry. 2009, 15: 637-646.
Poliak S, Peles E: The local differentiation of myelinated axons at nodes of Ranvier. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003, 4: 968-980. 10.1038/nrn1253.
Kjelgaard MM, Tager-Flusberg H: An investigation of language impairment in autism: implications for genetic subgroups. Lang Cogn Process. 2001, 16: 287-308. 10.1080/01690960042000058.
Bishop DVM: Autism and specific language impairment: categorical distinction or continuum?. Novartis Found Symp. 2003, 251: 213-226. Discussion 226-234, 281-297
Baird J, Stevenson JC, Williams DC: The evolution of ADHD: a disorder of communication?. Q Rev Biol. 2000, 75: 17-35. 10.1086/393256.
Redmond SM: Conversational profiles of children with ADHD, SLI and typical development. Clin Linguist Phon. 2004, 18: 107-125. 10.1080/02699200310001611612.
Whitehouse AJ, Barry JG, Bishop DVM: The broader language phenotype of autism: a comparison with specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007, 48: 822-830. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01765.x.
Bishop DVM, Baird G: Parent and teacher report of pragmatic aspects of communication: use of the children’s communication checklist in a clinical setting. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001, 43: 809-818. 10.1017/S0012162201001475.
The SLI Consortium: Highly significant linkage to the SLI1 locus in an expanded sample of individuals affected by specific language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2004, 74: 1225-1238.
Monaco AP: Multivariate linkage analysis of specific language impairment (SLI). Ann Hum Genet. 2007, 71 (Pt 5): 660-673
Falcaro M, Pickles A, Newbury DF, Addis L, Banfield E, Fisher SE, Monaco AP, Simkin Z, Conti-Ramsden G: Genetic and phenotypic effects of phonological short-term memory and grammatical morphology in specific language impairment. Genes Brain Behav. 2008, 7: 393-402. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00364.x.
Anderson CA, Pettersson FH, Clarke GM, Cardon LR, Morris AP, Zondervan KT: Data quality control in genetic case-control association studies. Nat Protoc. 2010, 5: 1564-1573. 10.1038/nprot.2010.116.
Dilthey AT, Moutsianas L, Leslie S, McVean G: HLA*IMP-an integrated framework for imputing classical HLA alleles from SNP genotypes. Bioinformatics. 2011, 27: 968-972. 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr061.
Schreuder GM, Hurley CK, Marsh SG, Lau M, Maiers M, Kollman C, Noreen HJ: The HLA dictionary 2001: a summary of HLA-A, -B, -C, -DRB1/3/4/5, -DQB1 alleles and their association with serologically defined HLA-A, -B, -C, -DR, and -DQ antigens. Hum Immunol. 2001, 62: 826-849. 10.1016/S0198-8859(01)00271-3.
Palmer CG, Hsieh HJ, Reed EF, Lonnqvist J, Peltonen L, Woodward JA, Sinsheimer JS: HLA-B maternal-fetal genotype matching increases risk of schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet. 2006, 79: 710-715. 10.1086/507829.
Winney B, Boumertit A, Day T, Davison D, Echeta C, Evseeva I, Hutnik K, Leslie S, Nicodemus K, Royrvik EC, Tonks S, Yang X, Cheshire J, Longley P, Mateos P, Groom A, Relton C, Bishop DT, Black K, Northwood E, Parkinson L, Frayling TM, Steele A, Sampson JR, King T, Dixon R, Middleton D, Jennings B, Bowden R, Donnelly P, Bodmer W: People of the British Isles: preliminary analysis of genotypes and surnames in a UK-control population. Eur J Hum Genet. 2012, 20: 203-210. 10.1038/ejhg.2011.127.
Semel EM, Wiig EH, Secord W: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals - Revised. 1992, San Antonio: Phychological Corporation
Gathercole SE, Willis CS, Baddeley AD, Emslie H: The Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. Memory. 1994, 2: 103-127. 10.1080/09658219408258940.
Bishop DV, North T, Donlan C: Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996, 37: 391-403. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01420.x.
Archibald LM, Joanisse MF: On the sensitivity and specificity of nonword repetition and sentence recall to language and memory impairments in children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2009, 52: 899-914. 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0099).
Abecasis GR, Cherny SS, Cookson WO, Cardon LR: Merlin-rapid analysis of dense genetic maps using sparse gene flow trees. Nat Genet. 2002, 30: 97-101. 10.1038/ng786.
Abecasis GR, Cardon LR, Cookson WO: A general test of association for quantitative traits in nuclear families. Am J Hum Genet. 2000, 66: 279-292. 10.1086/302698.
Fairfax BP, Makino S, Radhakrishnan J, Plant K, Leslie S, Dilthey A, Ellis P, Langford C, Vannberg FO, Knight JC: Genetics of gene expression in primary immune cells identifies cell type-specific master regulators and roles of HLA alleles. Nat Genet. 2012, 44: 502-510. 10.1038/ng.2205.
Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, Bender D, Maller J, Sklar P, de Bakker PI, Daly MJ, Sham PC: PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007, 81: 559-575. 10.1086/519795.
Carlson JM, Heckerman D, Shani G: Estimating false discovery rates for contingency tables. Microsoft Research technical report, MSR-TR-2009-53. 2009
Morris JA, Gardner MJ: Calculating confidence intervals for relative risks (odds ratios) and standardised ratios and rates. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 1988, 296: 1313-1316. 10.1136/bmj.296.6632.1313.
Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y: Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol. 1995, 57: 289-300.
Storey JD: A direct approach to false discovery rates. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B Statistical Methodology. 2002, 64: 479-498. 10.1111/1467-9868.00346.
Verhoeven KJF, Simonsen KL, McIntyre LM: Implementing false discovery rate control: increasing your power. Oikos. 2005, 108: 643-647. 10.1111/j.0030-1299.2005.13727.x.
Ramagopalan SV, Herrera BM, Bell JT, Dyment DA, Deluca GC, Lincoln MR, Orton SM, Chao MJ, Sadovnick AD, Ebers GC: Parental transmission of HLA-DRB1*15 in multiple sclerosis. Hum Genet. 2008, 122: 661-663. 10.1007/s00439-007-0442-z.
Wang Y-p, Tian Y, Zhu J-h, Yang Y-f, Zhang H-b, Wang C-h, Liu L, LV Y, Xiong L-p: Study on the association between HLA-DRB1 genes and ADHD in Xi’an. Chinese Journal of Child Health Care. 2008, 16: 010-
Wright P, Donaldson PT, Underhill JA, Choudhuri K, Doherty DG, Murray RM: Genetic association of the HLA DRB1 gene locus on chromosome 6p21.3 with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1996, 153: 1530-1533.
Odell JD, Warren RP, Warren WL, Burger RA, Maciulis A: Association of genes within the major histocompatibility complex with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychobiology. 1997, 35: 181-186. 10.1159/000119342.
Torres AR, Maciulis A, Stubbs EG, Cutler A, Odell D: The transmission disequilibrium test suggests that HLA-DR4 and DR13 are linked to autism spectrum disorder. Hum Immunol. 2002, 63: 311-316. 10.1016/S0198-8859(02)00374-9.
Lee LC, Zachary AA, Leffell MS, Newschaffer CJ, Matteson KJ, Tyler JD, Zimmerman AW: HLA-DR4 in families with autism. Pediatr Neurol. 2006, 35: 303-307. 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2006.06.006.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all the families, professionals, and individuals who participated in this research. In particular, we would like to thank Simon Fiddy for his assistance with data transformation and Benjamin Fairfax for the use of the Oxfordshire Control samples. DN is a Medical Research Council (MRC) Career Development Fellow and a Junior Research Fellow at St John’s College, University of Oxford. The work of the DN lab is funded by the MRC (G1000569/1 and MR/J003719/1). RN is funded by a University of Oxford Nuffield Department of Medicine Prize Studentship. The genotyping of samples was funded by the Max Planck Society. The collection of the SLIC samples was supported by the Wellcome Trust (060774 and 076566). Recruitment of controls for the Oxfordshire study of gene expression in primary immune cells was supported by the Wellcome Trust (074318 and 088891), the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) (281824), and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Oxford Biomedical Research Centre. Recruitment of controls for the POBI study was supported by the Wellcome Trust (072974, 088262). PFB is supported by an NIHR (UK) Senior Investigator award and the Biomedical Research Centre in Mental Health at the South London & Maudsley NHS Trust Hospital, London. The work of the Wellcome Trust Centre in Oxford is supported by the Wellcome Trust (090532/Z/09/Z).
We are very grateful to the other members of the SLIC for their contributions to this work: V. Slonims (Newcomen Centre, Evelina Children’s Hospital, London), A. Clark, J. Watson (Speech and Hearing Sciences, Queen Margaret University, Edinburgh, UK), E. Simonoff, A Pickles (King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry); A. Everitt (University Child Health and DMDE, University of Aberdeen); J. Seckl (Molecular Medicine Centre, University of Edinburgh); H. Cowie (Department of Speech and Language Therapy, Royal Hospital for Sick Children, Edinburgh); W. Cohen (Psychological Sciences and Health, University of Strathclyde); J. Nasir (Division of Biomedical Sciences, St George’s University of London); D.V.M. Bishop (Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford); Z. Simkin (School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Consortia
Corresponding author
Additional information
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors’ contributions
RN conceived and designed this study, performed the imputation and association analyses, and drafted the manuscript. NHS helped with the quality control for the DNA and generation of the SNP genotype data. GB managed the acquisition of data and DNA from the Guy’s Hospital SLIC cohort. AO managed the acquisition of data and DNA from the Edinburgh SLIC cohort. GC-R managed the acquisition of data and DNA from the Manchester SLIC cohort. PFB managed the acquisition of data and DNA from the Cambridge SLIC cohort. ERH managed the acquisition of data and DNA from the Aberdeen SLIC cohort; the SLIC is a group of individuals who collected the DNA and data for the SLIC cohort and provided vital intellectual input to the study design and management of the SLIC resource. APM is the principal investigator for the SLIC genetic data and assisted in the conceptualization of the study and contributed to the intellectual content of the manuscript. JCK managed the acquisition of data and DNA from the control individuals from the Oxfordshire study of gene expression in primary immune cells and assisted with the interpretation of the data for the manuscript. BW managed the acquisition of data and DNA from the control individuals for the POBI cohort and provided intellectual input into the study design. SEF managed the generation of SNP data for the SLIC individuals and contributed to the intellectual conception of this study. DFN performed quality-control procedures on the SLIC genetic data and helped with the conceptualization and design of the study and the drafting of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Electronic supplementary material
Authors’ original submitted files for images
Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
About this article
Cite this article
Nudel, R., Simpson, N.H., Baird, G. et al. Associations of HLA alleles with specific language impairment. J Neurodevelop Disord 6, 1 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-6-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-6-1
Comments
View archived comments (1)