Skip to main content

Stunting and lead: using causal mediation analysis to better understand how environmental lead exposure affects cognitive outcomes in children

Abstract

Background

Many children in Bangladesh experience poor nutritional status and environmental lead exposure, both of which are associated with lower scores on neurodevelopmental assessments. Recent studies have suggested that part of lead’s adverse effects on neurodevelopment are caused in part by lead’s effect on growth. New statistical methods are now available to evaluate potential causal pathways in observational studies. This study used a novel statistical method to test the hypothesis that stunting, a measure of linear growth related to poor nutrition, is a mediator and/or an effect modifier of the lead exposure’s adverse effect on cognitive development.

Methods

Participants were 734 children from a longitudinal birth cohort established in rural Bangladesh to study the health effects of prenatal and early childhood environmental metal exposures. Lead exposure was estimated using umbilical cord blood samples obtained at birth and blood obtained via venipuncture at age 20–40 months. Stunting was determined using the World Health Organization’s standards. Neurodevelopment was assessed at age 20–40 months years using the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition (BSID-III). We evaluated the effect of lead on stunting and whether the effect of lead on cognitive scores is modified by stunting status in multivariable regression analyses. We then conducted a novel 4-way mediation analysis that allows for exposure-mediator interaction to assess how much of the effect of lead on cognitive scores is explained by the pathway through stunting (mediation) and how much is explained by the interaction between lead and stunt (effect modification).

Results

Stunting was not a mediator of the effect of lead in our analyses. Results suggested effect modification by stunting. In an area of Bangladesh with lower lead exposures (median umbilical cord blood lead concentration, 1.7 μg/dL), stunting modified the relationship between prenatal blood lead concentrations and cognitive score at age 2–3 years. A 1-unit increase in natural log cord blood lead concentration in the presence of stunting was associated with a 2.1-unit decrease in cognitive scores (β = − 2.10, SE = 0.71, P = 0.003). This interaction was not found in a second study site where lead exposures were higher (median umbilical cord blood lead concentration, 6.1 μg/dL, β = − 0.45, SE = 0.49, P = 0.360).

Conclusions

We used a novel method of mediation analysis to test whether stunting mediated the adverse effect of prenatal lead exposure on cognitive outcomes in Bangladesh. While we did not find that stunting acted as mediator of lead’s effect on cognitive development, we found significant effect modification by stunting. Our results suggest that children with stunting are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of low-level lead exposure.

Introduction

Environmental lead exposure is associated with a wide range of neurodevelopmental deficits in children, with no threshold below which lead does not have adverse effects [1,2,3,4,5,6,7] Chronic malnutrition is also associated with cognitive deficits [8,9,10,11]. Children who experience food insecurity and are subjected to environmental contaminants, such as metals, may be particularly prone to neurodevelopmental delays and other negative health outcomes [12].

One common indicator of a nutritional deficit is stunted growth, which occurs when a child’s height (or length) for a given age is below the 5th percentile [13]. Current estimates show that the world’s poorest children have stunting rates more than double those of the richest [14, 15]. Similar trends are seen in the global burden of lead exposure, with children in developing countries at the greatest risk of exposure to this heavy metal [16]. Such findings highlight the strong likelihood of concurrent exposures, yet to our knowledge, only one study has investigated the joint effects and interactions of co-exposure to lead and stunting on neurodevelopment; no significant stunting-by-metal interactions for lead, arsenic, and manganese were reported [12].

Children in Bangladesh experience both stunting and exposure to metals [12, 17]. Thirty-six percent of children under the age of 5 years in Bangladesh are stunted [11], and stunting rates within the country are higher as socioeconomic status declines [17]. At the district administrative level, stunting rates range from 28 to 51%, and over half of the districts in Bangladesh (39/64) have stunting rates greater than the World Health Organization’s (WHO) critical threshold of 40% [18]. Blood metal levels, such as blood lead, are not surveyed nationally; however, previous research found that 78% of children from urban and rural areas had blood lead levels above the CDC reference level of 5 μg/dL [19] and 87% of children at a primary school in Dhaka had BLLs ≥ 10 μg/dL [20]. In contrast, approximately 97.5% of children in the USA have blood lead levels below the current reference level of 5 μg/dL [21].

Stunting may be a mediator of the lead exposure-cognitive development relationship, or part of the causal pathway. Many studies have shown an inverse correlation between blood lead concentration and children’s height [19, 22, 23], with postulation that lead causes decreases of gonadotropin secretion, and that abnormalities in growth hormone axis can contribute to adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes [24]. Stunting might also act as an effect modifier that is the adverse effect of lead is greater among children with stunting. One potential mechanism for this is increased absorption of lead: children with compromised nutritional status may absorb more metals into their bodies compared to children with adequate nutritional intake, leading to greater declines in neurodevelopment [25, 26]. Additionally, both lead and malnutrition affect neuronal metabolism. The combination could lead to greater declines in neurodevelopment. Understanding whether growth is an effect modifier or mediator is important to design effective interventions to improve neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Mediation analysis is an established method used in social and epidemiological studies to understand the impact of variables in causal pathways or biological mechanisms. New statistical methods have been developed to understand the contributions of effect modification and mediation in epidemiological studies using a causal framework [27,28,29]. Using mediation analysis, the effect of lead can be decomposed into the component due to only mediation through stunting (the pure indirect effect), only interaction between lead and stunting (the reference interaction), both (the mediated interaction), or neither (the controlled direct effect) [30]. Past research in this cohort has found negative effects of metals on neurodevelopment, both in individual concentrations and in mixtures [30].

The aim of this study was to evaluate the role of stunting on the relationship between lead exposure and neurodevelopment scores using a Bangladeshi birth cohort. Our previous studies from this cohort revealed an association between prenatal lead exposure and adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes in early childhood [31], a finding that is consistent with decades of research. We have also used data from this cohort to understand the effect of metal mixtures on neurodevelopment [30, 32], to apply new statistical methods for the study of multi-pollutant mixtures [33], and to validate gene-environment studies [34]. In this analysis, we hypothesize that stunting acts as a mediator of environmental lead exposure. If our hypothesis is correct, one implication is that interventions targeted to improve chronic malnutrition would be effective in reducing the adverse effects of environmental lead exposure. Our previous studies have shown lead is associated with decreased cognitive scores in children and its effect is greater than those of other environmental metals. We have also shown that when blood lead levels are high, lead is associated with decreased cognitive scores and effects of other metals are not detected [31]. Therefore, in this study, we focus on lead rather than on environmental mixtures.

Methods

The study population has been described previously [30, 31, 35, 36]. Briefly, between 2008 and 2011, pregnant women in the Sirajdikhan (alternate spelling Serajdikhan) and Pabna upazilas (administrative regions) of Bangladesh served by Dhaka Community Hospital (DCH) were enrolled and followed through pregnancy and delivery. Children were followed at birth, age 12 months, and age 20–40 months.

The research team obtained informed consent from the parents of all children. The project was approved by the Human Research Committees at the Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health (HSPH) and DCH. The Committee for Clinical Investigation at Boston Children’s Hospital (BCH) ceded review of this study to HSPH.

We enrolled 1613 pregnant women in the birth cohort study. Of these, 1153 pregnancies resulted in singleton live births and 5 pregnancies resulted in twin births. Of the remaining women, 123 were lost to follow-up, 125 withdrew from study activities, 132 pregnancies resulted in miscarriage, and 75 pregnancies resulted in stillbirth. A total of 1153 women and their children participated in follow-up studies, including 815 children who participated in neurodevelopmental assessments at age 20–40 months. Complete data were available for 734 children, and this is the sample used for analysis.

Environmental exposure assessment

Exposure to lead, arsenic, and manganese was assessed using blood samples collected at two time points: at birth using the umbilical cord blood and at 20–40 months of age collected via venipuncture, representing early childhood exposure. We collected the blood in trace metal-free tubes. The umbilical cord blood samples were analyzed at the Trace Metals Laboratory at HSPH using ICP-MS. Venous blood samples were analyzed at HSPH and the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai using similar protocols [30]. The average limit of detection (LOD) for lead and arsenic was 0.01 μg/dL; the LOD for manganese was 0.05 μg/dL.

Stunting

Trained field staff measured the weight, length, and head circumference of the newborns and children [32]. We measured height and head circumference to the nearest 0.1 cm and weight to the nearest 0.1 kg. We calculated z-scores for child’s height-for-age using the WHO’s Multicentre Growth Reference Study (MGRS) [13]. These standards provide a single international standard of physiological growth for all children from birth to 5 years of age. Specific computations were performed with the WHO Anthro and Macros (Version 3.2.2). Children with height-for-age less than or equal to 2 z-scores below the median of the WHO’s child growth standards were classified as stunted.

Neurodevelopmental outcomes

We translated the Bayley Scales of infant and Toddler Development™, Third Edition (BSID-III™) and adapted it for use in rural Bangladesh [37]. The BSID-III was administered by trained study personnel blinded to participants’ environmental exposure measures. The quality control included review of all BSID-III videotapes by site leaders (Ibne Hasan and Halder), frequent observed assessments (Mazumdar), and review of 5% of the videotaped administrations of the BSID-III by a senior neuropsychologist (Bellinger).

Covariates

We collected demographic information using structured questionnaires to obtain data on age, education, tobacco exposure, socioeconomic status, and dietary information [19]. Maternal height and weight were recorded at the initial study enrollment visit during the 1st trimester of pregnancy. Maternal education was recoded and collapsed into a dichotomous variable (primary education or less/more than a primary education) due to small numbers in the higher education categories. This categorization is consistent with other studies from Bangladesh [38,39,40]. We used a locally validated dish-based semi-quantitative food questionnaire (FFQ) to collect dietary information [41]. A measure of maternal protein intake during pregnancy was derived from the FFQ and coded to indicate weekly protein consumption of fish, meat, and eggs as high (> 26 units), medium (12.5–26 units), or low (< 12.5 units). Interviewers also administered the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) Inventory [42]. This inventory was previously translated and adapted for use in Bangladesh [43]. Mothers’ IQ was estimated using Raven’s Progressive Indices [44].

Statistical methods

We performed statistical analyses using RStudio version 1.2.5033 (using R version 4.0.1) [45] and Stata version 16 [46]. We examined distributional plots and descriptive statistics for all variables and performed additional analyses stratified by clinic site. We evaluated for potential selection bias by comparing baseline characteristics of the birth cohort to those of the study sample. We modeled blood metal concentrations as natural log-transformed continuous variables. This transformation achieves a common scale and accounts for the right-skewedness of the biomarker levels. One study ID had a blood sample below the LOD for venipuncture lead, arsenic, and manganese at the 20–40 months of visit. We used one half of the LOD for this observation. Cognitive scores were approximately normally distributed and were modeled as continuous outcomes.

Mediation analysis

We conducted mediation analysis to evaluate whether stunting was acting as a mediator of the relationship between cord blood lead concentration and cognitive test scores in early childhood using the 4-way decomposition proposed by VanderWeele (Fig. 1). The mediation analysis was conducted using the med4way command in Stata [28, 32], a package that our group developed previously for mediation analysis. For this specific study, we tested the effect of a change in cord blood lead from the 25th to the 75th percentile. As stunting was a dichotomous variable, the parameter m, denoting the level of the mediator, was set to 0 to estimate the controlled direct effect (the effect of lead that is neither due to mediation nor interaction). The analyses require adjustment for exposure-outcome, exposure-mediator, mediator-outcome confounders. All other covariates were held at their respective means. Using this mediation analysis, the effect of lead can be decomposed into the component due to only mediation through stunting (the pure indirect effect), only interaction between lead and stunting (the reference interaction), both (the mediated interaction), or neither (the controlled direct effect) [30]. Mean, median, and confidence interval estimates for each component of the decomposition were obtained for each site.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Diagram showing the mediation model components testing the effect of cord blood lead on cognitive score with stunting status as a mediator, following a previous directed acyclic graph [28]

The covariates used in the model were selected to meet the criteria for using the mediation analysis [28], including all confounders of exposure-mediator, exposure-outcome, and mediator-outcome are adjusted for in the model. Additionally, there must be no mediator-outcome confounders affected by the exposure. Unadjusted models were analyzed for each of these components and possible confounders were added to a subsequent adjusted model. New adjusted models were created for each variable of interest. We assumed a variable was a confounder is the effect estimate for the predictor changed by greater than 10%.

Regression models and interaction analyses

Mediation analysis requires the specification of a model for the outcome (cognitive score) and a model for the mediator (stunting). A multivariable linear regression model was used to model the mean of cognitive score conditionally on cord blood lead, stunting, and confounders. A logistic regression model was used to model the conditional odds of stunting score given cord blood lead and confounders. The following covariates were included in models to evaluate for confounding the study site, child’s sex, child’s birth length, child birth weight, child’s age at the time of neurodevelopmental testing, mother’s age at the time of the child’s birth, maternal education, maternal IQ, maternal height and weight, HOME score, secondhand smoke exposure, maternal protein intake, cord blood arsenic and manganese, and early childhood blood metal concentrations. To identify if squared terms were needed in the regression model, we used the generalized additive model (gam) spline function in R’s mgcv package (Mixed GAM computation vehicle) [47]. The criteria for remaining in the model consisted of a p value less than 0.05 and an effective degrees of freedom (EDF) greater than 1. Given the important differences in exposure profiles and baseline characteristics observed across the two study sites, regression models were stratified by clinic in primary analyses and adjusted for in secondary analyses. We evaluated for effect modification by stunting status using interaction terms in the regression models. We performed additional analyses using the z-score length-for-age as a continuous variable for child growth.

Results

Study population

A summary of the study population is presented in Table 1. In general, the population was primarily agricultural/rural with low rates of formal education for mothers. There were important differences between the sites. Children from Sirajdikhan had higher blood lead concentrations in cord blood samples (median 6.1 vs. 1.7 μg/dL) and at 20–40 months (median 8.2 vs. 4.0 μg/dL) than children from Pabna. Children from Sirajdikhan also had higher rates of stunting than children from Pabna (66% vs. 40%), although the mothers in Sirjadikhan were generally heavier (median 47 vs. 45 kg). Mothers in Pabna were more likely to consume high amounts of protein during pregnancy compared to mothers in Sirajdikhan (44% vs 3%). We did not find significant differences in baseline characteristics between the full original cohort and the complete case study sample used in this analysis.

Table 1 Study population

Mediation analysis

We performed stratified analyses by site due to concerns about residual confounding, a strategy that is consistent with other reports from this cohort [31, 48]. A table reporting the 4 decomposed effects is shown in Table 2. In Pabna, where lead exposure was generally lower, the mediation analysis demonstrated a significant effect resulting from the reference interaction (the effect due only to interaction) between cord blood lead concentration and stunting, which reduced cognitive scores − 0.779 (p = 0.001). We did not find evidence of a reference interaction in Sirajdikhan (p = 0.483). No significant effect was found for mediation only, mediation and interaction combined, or neither mediation nor interaction in either site. Collectively, these results suggest the presence of effect modification by stunting in an area with low-level lead exposure.

Table 2 Mediation analysis of the effect of log cord blood lead concentrations on cognitive scores when mediated by stunting status at both study sites

Effect modification

In our regression models, we confirmed a significant interaction between lead and stunting status, both in the cohort as a whole and in our stratified analyses. In the 3-way model for the entire cohort, study site had a main effect on cognitive scores (β = − 2.77, SE = 1.15, p = 0.016) with Sirajdikhan coded as study site 0 and Pabna coded as study site 1. The results for the 3-way interaction confirmed a significant interaction for log cord blood lead (μg/dL)*stunting*study site (β = − 1.67, SE = 0.89, p = 0.06) on cognitive scores. In our stratified models, in Pabna, an area where lead levels were low, we found a 1-unit increase in natural log cord blood lead μg/dL in the presence of stunting was associated with a 2.10 decrease in cognitive scores (p = 0.003, standard error = 0.713). In Sirajdikhan, where blood lead concentrations were generally higher, this interaction was not found (Table 3). In sensitivity analysis, we found a significant interaction of log cord blood lead (μg/dL)*z-score height-for-age in Pabna (β = 0.86, SE = 0.39, p = 0.03) and results that approached statistical significance in Sirajdikhan (β = 0.42, SE = 0.22, p = 0.06) (Table 3). These findings confirm our hypothesis that child growth is an important effect modifier of lead.

Table 3 Regression results for the effect of cord blood lead concentration and childhood stunting on cognitive neurodevelopment at study sites

Discussion

In this study, we applied causal mediation analysis to assess the contribution of stunting, a measure of impaired growth related to poor nutrition, to the adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes associated with environmental lead exposure. We found that stunting modifies the adverse effects of lead, such that the adverse effect of lead was greater among children who were stunted. Effect modification was most apparent among children with low-level lead exposure. The results suggest that a child experiencing stunting with a log-cord blood lead concentration of 4 μg/dL may have cognitive scores 8.4 points lower than a child with similar cord blood lead exposure but without stunting. Our analyses did not reveal evidence of mediation, suggesting that stunting itself it does not lie within the causal pathway between lead exposure and adverse cognitive development.

Dietary interventions have long been part of a comprehensive strategy to prevent and mitigate the neurodevelopmental effects of environmental lead exposure in children [49]. These strategies are based on observations that blood lead is associated with lower intake of iron [50], calcium [51], vitamin C [52], and D[53] and that blood lead concentration among children is associated decreased height of children [22]. These observational studies are limited in determining causality and in answering important questions needed to design effective interventions [54, 55].

In this study, we apply a novel statistical method that simultaneously evaluates for mediation and effect modification [27, 28]. In our example, while we observe that stunted children are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of low-level, we also determine that stunting is unlikely to be part of the mechanism by which lead causes adverse cognitive development. These results suggest that resources might best be used to screen for lead exposure among populations with high rates of stunting and reduce lead exposure in these populations.

Strengths and limitations

Our studies used biomarkers of metal exposure using ICP-MS, a gold standard for the measure of blood lead concentration and a measure not widely available in low-resource settings such as Bangladesh. In addition to lead, we were able to control for exposure to other toxic metals in our models, which is an additional strength. Understanding the effects of metal mixtures was not the focus of this analysis. Additionally, we were able to adjust for a large number of potential confounders as well as predictors of stunting and neurodevelopment. However, we recognized the potential for unmeasured confounding variables and stratified analyses by clinic reduced the heterogeneity in demographics and potential for residual confounding.

Conclusion

Causal mediation analysis enables investigation of potential mechanisms of disease in epidemiological studies. We used a novel method of mediation analysis to test whether stunting, a measure height-for-age related to poor nutrition, mediated the adverse effect of prenatal lead exposure on cognitive outcomes in Bangladesh. While we did not find that stunting acted as mediator, we found significant effect modification by stunting. Our study suggests that stunted children are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of low-level lead exposure, interventions that include targeting populations with high rates of stunting for lead screening and reduction of lead exposure are most likely to be effective in improving neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from David Christiani, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, dchris@hsph.harvard.edu.

Abbreviations

BCH:

Boston Children’s Hospital

HSPH:

Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health

BSID-III:

Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition

DCH:

Dhaka Community Hospital

FFQ:

Food Frequency Questionnaire

HOME:

Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment Inventory

LMIC:

Low- to middle-income country

LOD:

Limit of detection

mgcv:

Mixed GAM Computation Vehicle

MGRS:

Multicentre Growth Reference Study

SD:

Standard deviation

WHO:

World Health Organization

References

  1. Plusquellec P, Muckle G, Dewailly E, Ayotte P, Jacobson S, Jacobson J. The relation of low-level prenatal lead exposure to behavioral indicators of attention in Inuit infants in Arctic Quebec. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2007;29(5):527–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Jedrychowski W, Perera FP, Jankowski J, Mrozek-Budzyn D, Mroz E, Flak E, et al. Very low prenatal exposure to lead and mental development of children in infancy and early childhood. Neuroepidemiology. 2009;32(4):270–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Beltrán-Aguilar ED, Barker LK, Canto MT, Dye BA, Gooch BF, Griffin SO, et al. Surveillance for dental caries, dental sealants, tooth retention, edentulism, and enamel fluorosis--United States, 1988-1994 and 1999-2002. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2005;54(3):1–43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Ettinger AS, Ruckart PZ, Dignam T. Lead poisoning prevention: the unfinished agenda. J Public Health Manage Pract. 2019;25:S1–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Whitehead LS, Buchanan SD. Childhood lead poisoning: a perpetual environmental justice issue? J Public Health Manag Pract. 2019;25:S115–S20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bellinger DC, Stiles KM, Needleman HL. Low-level lead exposure, intelligence and academic achievement: a long-term follow-up study. Pediatrics. 1992;90(6):855–61.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Thomason ME, Hect JL, Rauh VA, Trentacosta C, Wheelock MD, Eggebrecht AT, et al. Prenatal lead exposure impacts cross-hemispheric and long-range connectivity in the human fetal brain. Neuroimage. 2019;191:186–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Grantham-McGregor S. A review of studies of the effect of severe malnutrition on mental development. J Nutr. 1995;125(8 Suppl):2233S–8S.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kar BR, Rao SL, Chandramouli BA. Cognitive development in children with chronic protein energy malnutrition. Behav Brain Funct. 2008;4:12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Chang SM, Walker SP, Grantham-McGregor S, Powell CA. Early childhood stunting and later behaviour and school achievement. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43(6):775–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Nahar B, Hossain M, Mahfuz M, Islam MM, Hossain MI, Murray-Kolb LE, et al. Early childhood development and stunting-findings from MAL-ED birth cohort study in Bangladesh. Matern Child Nutr. 2019:e12864.

  12. Wasserman GA, Liu X, Factor-Litvak P, Gardner JM, Graziano JH. Developmental impacts of heavy metals and undernutrition. Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol. 2008;102(2):212–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group. WHO Child Growth Standards: Length/height-for-age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age: methods and development. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  14. De Onis M, Brown D, Blossner M, Borghi E. Levels and trends in child malnutrition. In: UNICEF-WHO-The World Bank joint child malnutrition estimates; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  15. UNICEF. The state of the world’s children: a fair chance for every child. New York: UNICEF; 2016. https://www.unicef.org/publications/index_91711.html.

  16. World Health Organization. The world health report 2002: reducing risks, promoting healthy life. World Health Organization; 2002. https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/42510/WHR_2002.pdf?sequence=1.

  17. Islam A, Biswas T. Chronic stunting among under-5 children in Bangladesh: a situation analysis. Adv Pediatr Res. 2015;2:18.

    Google Scholar 

  18. World Food Programme. Undernutrition Maps of Bangladesh 2012. Dhaka: Bangladesh Bureau of Statistics (BBS)/UN World Food Programme (WFP); 2012.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  19. Gleason KM, Valeri L, Shankar AH, Hasan MO, Quamruzzaman Q, Rodrigues EG, et al. Stunting is associated with blood lead concentration among Bangladeshi children aged 2-3 years. Environ Health. 2016;15(1):103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Kaiser R, Henderson AK, Daley WR, Naughton M, Khan MH, Rahman M, et al. Blood lead levels of primary school children in Dhaka, Bangladesh. Environ Health Perspect. 2001;109(6):563–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Paulson JA, Brown MJ. The CDC blood lead reference value for children: time for a change. Environ Health. 2019;18(1):16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Schwartz J, Angle C, Pitcher H. Relationship between childhood blood lead levels and stature. Pediatrics (Evanston). 1986;77(3):281–8.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Kafourou A, Touloumi C, Makropoulos V, Loutradi A, Papanagiotou A, Hatzakis A. Effects of lead on the somatic growth of children. Arch Environ Health. 1997;52(5):377–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kaji M, Nishi Y. Lead and Growth. Clin Pediatr Endocrinol. 2006;15(4):123–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Mahaffey KR. Nutrition and lead: strategies for public health. Environ Health Perspect. 1995;103(Suppl 6):191–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Kordas K, Lönnerdal B, Stoltzfus RJ. Interactions between nutrition and environmental exposures: effects on health outcomes in women and children. J Nutr. 2007;137(12):2794–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. VanderWeele TJ. A unification of mediation and interaction: a 4-way decomposition. Epidemiology. 2014;25(5):749–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Discacciati A, Bellavia A, Lee JJ, Mazumdar M, Valeri L. Med4way: a Stata command to investigate mediating and interactive mechanisms using the four-way effect decomposition. Int J Epidemiol. 2018;48:(1):15-20. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyy236.

  29. Linda Valeri TJV. Mediation analysis allowing for exposure–mediator interactions and causal interpretation: theoretical assumptions and implementation with SAS and SPSS macros. Psychol Methods. 2013;18(2):137-50. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031034.

  30. Valeri L, Mazumdar MM, Bobb JF, Claus Henn B, Rodrigues E, Sharif OIA, et al. The joint effect of prenatal exposure to metal mixtures on neurodevelopmental outcomes at 20-40 months of age: evidence from rural Bangladesh. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125(6):067015.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Rodrigues EG, Bellinger DC, Valeri L, Hasan MO, Quamruzzaman Q, Golam M, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes among 2- to 3-year-old children in Bangladesh with elevated blood lead and exposure to arsenic and manganese in drinking water. Environ Health. 2016;15:44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Lee JJ, Valeri L, Kapur K, Ibne Hasan MOS, Quamruzzaman Q, Wright RO, et al. Growth parameters at birth mediate the relationship between prenatal manganese exposure and cognitive test scores among a cohort of 2- to 3-year-old Bangladeshi children. Int J Epidemiol. 2018;47(4):1169–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Bobb JF, Valeri L, Claus Henn B, Christiani DC, Wright RO, Mazumdar M, et al. Bayesian kernel machine regression for estimating the health effects of multi-pollutant mixtures. Biostatistics. 2015;16(3):493–508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Sun R, Wang Z, Claus Henn B, Su L, Lu Q, Lin X, et al. Identification of novel loci associated with infant cognitive ability. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;25(11):3010–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Kile ML, Rodrigues EG, Mazumdar M, Dobson CB, Diao N, Golam M, et al. A prospective cohort study of the association between drinking water arsenic exposure and self-reported maternal health symptoms during pregnancy in Bangladesh. Environ Health. 2014;13(1):29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Lee JJ, Kapur K, Rodrigues EG, Ibne Hasan MOS, Quamruzzaman Q, Wright RO, et al. Anthropometric measures at birth and early childhood are associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes among Bangladeshi children aged 2-3 years. Sci Total Environ. 2017;607-608:475–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-Third Edition. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Harshfield E, Chowdhury R, Harhay MN, Bergquist H, Harhay MO. Association of hypertension and hyperglycaemia with socioeconomic contexts in resource-poor settings: the Bangladesh Demographic and Health Survey. Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44(5):1625–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Hashizume M, Wagatsuma Y, Faruque AS, Hayashi T, Hunter PR, Armstrong B, et al. Factors determining vulnerability to diarrhoea during and after severe floods in Bangladesh. J Water Health. 2008;6(3):323–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Nasrin S, Rasiah R, Baskaran A, Khan HTA. The relationship between conversion factors and health: evidence from the ready-made garment workers in Bangladesh. J Asian Afr Stud. 2018;54(1):20–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Lin P-I, Bromage S, Mostofa MG, Allen J, Oken E, Kile ML, et al. Validation of a dish-based semiquantitative food questionnaire in rural Bangladesh. Nutrients. 2017;9(1):49.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Totsika V, Sylva K. The home observation for measurement of the environment revisited. Child Adolesc Mental Health. 2004;9(1):25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Black MM, Baqui AH, Zaman K, Persson LA, El Arifeen S, Le K, et al. Iron and zinc supplementation promote motor development and exploratory behavior among Bangladeshi infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004;80(4):903–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Raven J. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press; 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Team RC. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  46. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 16. College Station: LLC S; 2019.

  47. Wood S. Generalized additive models : an introduction with R. 2nd ed. Philadelphia: CRC Press, LLC; 2017.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  48. Valeri L, Mazumdar MM, Bobb JF, Claus Henn B, Rodrigues E, Sharif OIA, et al. The joint effect of prenatal exposure to metal mixtures on neurodevelopmental outcomes at 20–40 months of age: evidence from rural Bangladesh. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;067015:1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Gulson B, Mizon K, Taylor A, Wu M. Dietary zinc, calcium and nickel are associated with lower childhood blood lead levels. Environ Res. 2019;168:439–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Asa B, Brenda E, Patrice S, Marcos A, Lynn RG. Iron deficiency associated with higher blood lead in children living in contaminated environments. Environ Health Perspect. 2001;109(10):1079–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Adrienne SE, Héctor L-F, Martha MT-R, Adriana M-G, Karen EP, Joel S, et al. Effect of calcium supplementation on blood lead levels in pregnancy: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Environ Health Perspect. 2009;117(1):26–31.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Simon JA, Hudes ES. Relationship of ascorbic acid to blood lead levels. JAMA. 1999;281(24):2289–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Erin NH, Heidi JK, Richard H, Richard W, Kim D, Bruce PL. Vitamin D receptor Fok1 Polymorphism and blood lead concentration in children. Environ Health Perspect. 2003;111(13):1665–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Kordas K. The “Lead Diet”: can dietary approaches prevent or treat lead exposure? J Pediatr. 2017;185:224–31 e1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Kordas K, Burganowski R, Roy A, Peregalli F, Baccino V, Barcia E, et al. Nutritional status and diet as predictors of children's lead concentrations in blood and urine. Environ Int. 2018;111:43–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to recognize the research staff at Dhaka Community Hospital for collecting all field data in these studies and the research staff at the HSPH Trace Metals Laboratory and the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai for analyzing the blood samples.

Funding

Kelsey Gleason was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) training grant T42 OH008416 and R01 ES026317. Additional support was provided by the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (NIEHS) grants R01 ES015533, P42 ES016454, and P30 ES000002, and Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institutes of Child Health and Human Development grant U54 HD090255.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KMG, LV, AHS, DCB, DCC, and MM designed the study, and reviewed and revised the manuscript. KMB drafted the initial manuscript. ROW participated in the study design and reviewed the manuscript. JFO updated the statistical analysis and reviewed the manuscript. MOSIH supervised the neurodevelopmental assessments. QQ and GM supervised the field activities, reviewed the manuscript, and participated in the study design. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

Not applicable.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maitreyi Mazumdar.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The parents of all child participants provided informed consent before enrollment. The Committee for Clinical Investigation at Boston Children’s Hospital (BCH) ceded review of this study to the Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health (HSPH). The Human Research Committees at HSPH and Dhaka Community Hospital (DCH) approved this study.

Consent for publication

Consent for publication was granted during in informed consent process.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gleason, K.M., Valeri, L., Shankar, A.H. et al. Stunting and lead: using causal mediation analysis to better understand how environmental lead exposure affects cognitive outcomes in children. J Neurodevelop Disord 12, 39 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-020-09346-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-020-09346-x

Keywords