Skip to main content

Temporal stability of parent-reported behavior problems in late talkers over 2 years: a prospective case-control study from toddlerhood to preschool age

Abstract

Background

Late talking (LT) in toddlers is a risk factor for language weakness that may interfere with the development of using language to regulate behavior and emotion and contribute to the development of behavior problems from early childhood. This study examined the temporal stability of parent-reported behavior problems among Mandarin-speaking LT toddlers from ages 2 to 4 in Taiwan.

Methods

Thirty-one LT and 31 typical language development (TLD) toddlers were assessed for their vocabulary production at age 2 with the Words and Sentences Forms of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Developmental Inventories Toddler Form (Taiwan version). Additionally, participants’ receptive and expressive language abilities were assessed using the receptive and expressive language subscales of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition. At age 4, the Child Language Disorder Scale-Revised was applied and included the two core subtests for auditory comprehension and expressive communication. At ages 2 and 4 years, behavior problems were assessed with the Child Behavior Checklist.

Results

There was a higher percentage of participants with persistent behavior problems among LT toddlers than among TLD toddlers. Moreover, toddlers with larger vocabularies were less likely to develop withdrawal behaviors by preschool age.

Conclusions

This study supported the temporal stability of parent-reported behavior problems among LT toddlers across early childhood. Early identification of and intervention for behavior problems associated with LT in toddlerhood is essential to alleviate their behavior problems later in preschool years.

Introduction

The term late talkers, i.e., late-talking (LT) toddlers, refers to children aged 18–35 months who experience delayed onset of language but have no other diagnosed disabilities or developmental delays in neurological, sensory, cognitive, or motor domains [1, 2]. LT toddlers represent approximately 10–15% of the toddler population [3, 4]. There is extensive evidence that LT is a risk factor for language weakness persisting into adolescence [5]. Language delays may interfere with the development of using (self-directed) language to regulate behaviors [6] and emotion [7, 8] and may contribute to the development of behavior problems from early childhood.

Multiple panel studies have reported behavior problems among late talkers in toddlerhood [9,10,11,12,13], and late talking has been shown to be a predictor for such problems in childhood and adulthood [3, 14,15,16,17]. Internalizing and externalizing behaviors may follow different developmental pathways [18]. Previous studies found that a 2-year-old children with low receptive language abilities were at risk of internalizing behaviors [19, 20]. However, whether LT toddlers with behavior problems in later developmental stages (e.g., preschool age) show temporal stability from early behavior problems was unclear.

In fact, there has been very little panel research examining whether LT toddlers with behavior problems in later developmental stages (e.g., preschool age) show temporal stability from early behavior problems. Temporal stability—also known as developmental continuity—of behavior problems refers to a persistent presence of behavior problems over time. Temporal stability of early behavior problems has been found in typical language development (TLD) children; furthermore, temporal stability of behavior problems emerging in early life may be a predictor for the emergence of psychopathology [21, 22]. If the behavior problems of LT toddlers were stable across early childhood, early identification of and intervention for LT toddlers with behavior problems would be useful in reducing their risk of mental health problems later in life [23, 24].

This 2-year prospective case-control study examined the temporal stability of behavior problems from toddlerhood to preschool age in a community sample of LT and TLD toddlers. Based on developmental continuity and persistent poor language development in LT toddlers, we hypothesized that the percentage of LT toddlers with persistent behavior problems through early childhood would be higher than that of TLD toddlers with ongoing behavior problems. Moreover, we examined whether toddlers with larger vocabularies were less likely to have concurrent behavior problems (i.e., in toddlerhood) and develop them over time (i.e., by preschool age). These findings will deepen understanding of the associations between early language delay and temporal stability of behavior problems across various early developmental stages.

Methods

Participants

This prospective case-control cohort study comprised two waves of data collection. A total of 162 24–33-month-old toddlers were recruited from parenting websites or local pediatric clinics in northern Taiwan (Fig. 1). We assessed their expressive vocabulary using the Mandarin Chinese version of the Words and Sentences Forms of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Developmental Inventories Toddler Form (MCDI-T) [25] to screen toddlers with delayed expressive language. Out of 162 toddlers, 65 were included in the data collection at time 1. The matching criteria for LT toddlers included chronological age (within 1 month of birthdate), native language (Mandarin), birth order, and sex. Due to the strict matching criteria, 91 TLD participants who did not match LT participants on the criteria mentioned above and who completed only the MCDI-T were excluded from the final data analysis. Furthermore, one toddler diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and five toddlers with cognitive delays were also excluded. Among the 65 participants who completed both waves of data collection, 32 toddlers were in the LT group, and 33 were in the TLD group. The inclusion criteria for LT toddlers were the same as those in other studies [15, 26,27,28]. In particular, for the LT group, word production performance should be at or below the 15th percentile on the MCDI-T, whereas that for the TLD toddlers should be at or above the 25th percentile. Behavior problems of children were collected via parental reports [29]. One LT toddler and two TLD toddlers did not participate in subsequent follow-up (time 2) and were excluded. For the final data analysis of this longitudinal study, 31 LT toddlers (22 boys) and 31 TLD toddlers (22 boys) completed the data collection. The mean TLD and LT participant ages for the two data collection points were 27.59 (SD = 2.46) and 27.54 (SD = 2.60) months at time 1 and 50.88 (SD = 2.46) and 51.37 (SD = 2.53) months at time 2, respectively.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of study design, N = 62. Notes: ASD, autism spectrum disorders; CLDS-R, Child Language Disorder Scale-Revised; MCDI-T, Mandarin-Chinese version of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Developmental Inventories Toddler; M-CHAT, Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers

At time 1, the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) [30] was used as a screening tool to identify toddlers at high risk for ASD, and the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition (Bayley-III) [31] was used to identify children with cognitive delays. Furthermore, according to parental reports, all toddlers were born full term (gestational age > 36 weeks) with birth weights over 2500 g; no complications were encountered during pregnancy or delivery. Additionally, the participants had no other critical incidents, chronic diseases, or sensory-motor deficits.

Table 1 presents the participants’ demographic characteristics at age 2 (time 1). Although there were no differences between the TLD and LT groups (ps > .05) in demographic factors, 16.13% of TLD toddlers had a history of otitis media compared to 9.68% of LT toddlers. We also observed that few TLD or LT toddlers attended daycare. Approximately, 50% of TLD and LT toddlers had their parents as their primary caregivers. Over 70% of TLD and LT toddlers’ parents were educated at the university level and above, and over 50% of the toddlers were from middle- or high-income families. The two groups were similar in the history of otitis media, language exposure environment, and socioeconomic status.

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of participants at age 2

Table 2 presents participants’ cognitive and language scores at ages 2 (time 1) and 4 (time 2). No significant differences were noted in cognitive abilities by age in either of the two groups (ps > .05). The LT group exhibited lower receptive and expressive language skills than the TLD group at ages 2 [Fs(1, 60) = 16.48 and 143.65, respectively, ps < .001, ηp2s = .22 and .71] and 4 [Fs(1, 60) = 53.68 and 70.63, ps = .001, ηp2s = .47 and .54].

Table 2 Cognitive and language scores in the TLD and LT groups

Measures

Assessment of language abilities

We assessed the participants’ vocabulary production at age 2 with the Words and Sentences Forms of the MCDI-T [25], which has been used to assess toddlers aged 16–36 months. Additionally, the participants’ receptive and expressive language abilities at age 2 were assessed using the receptive and expressive language subscales of the Bayley-III, which has been used to assess toddlers aged 16–42 months [31].

The Mandarin Chinese version of the Child Language Disorder Scale-Revised (CLDS-R) [32] was employed to assess participants at age 4 and included the two core subtests for auditory comprehension and expressive communication. The CLDS-R has been used to assess children aged 3–6 years.

Assessment of behavior problems

At ages 2 and 4 years, behavior problems were assessed using the Mandarin Chinese version of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL-MC) for children aged 1.5 to 5 years [33]. Parents rated each item as 0 (not true), 1 (somewhat or sometimes true), or 2 (very true or often true). The scores were aggregated on eight syndrome subscales and three main scales. Scores on the emotionally reactive (e.g., “Disturbed by any change in routine”), anxious/depressed (e.g., “Unhappy, sad, or depressed”), somatic complaints (e.g., “Headaches (without medical cause)”), and withdrawn (e.g., “Shows little affection toward people”) subscales were aggregated to comprise the internalizing scale score. Similarly, the attention (e.g., “Can’t sit still, restless, or hyperactive”) and aggression (e.g., “Demands must be met immediately”) subscale scores were aggregated to comprise the externalizing scale score. Finally, the remaining two subscales, sleep problems (e.g., “Wakes up often at night”) and other problems, were combined with the internalizing and externalizing scales to comprise the total problems scale score. For the three main scales and seven syndrome subscales, except for the other problems syndrome subscale, raw scores could be converted to normalized T scores. The behavior problems status for the individual main scale and subscale scores were dichotomized as elevated (1 = the presence of behavior problems) or normal (0 = the absence of behavior problems). According to the CBCL’s definition, subscale scores below the 93rd percentile are considered normal, whereas scores at or above the 93rd percentile (T score ≥ 65) are considered elevated. For the major scales, scores below the 85th percentile are considered normal, whereas scores at or above the 85th percentile (T score ≥ 60) are considered elevated.

Assessment of cognitive abilities

The cognitive scale of the Bayley-III [31], administered in Mandarin Chinese, was employed to evaluate the participants’ cognitive abilities at age 2. This scale has been applied to children from birth to 42 months. At age 4 (i.e., over 42 months old), the participants’ cognitive abilities were measured using the Mandarin Chinese version of the Nonverbal Index (NVI) of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Fourth Edition (WPPSI-IV) [34]. This test was designed for assessments in children aged 2 years and 6 months to 7 years and 11 months.

Procedure

All tests were administered in a quiet room. Before testing, the researcher informed the parents of the research procedures, after which the parents provided their informed consent. The test administration duration for each wave was 1–1.5 h. Furthermore, the Bayley-III, the WPPSI-IV, and the CLDS-R tests were administered by a licensed clinical psychologist specializing in developmental psychopathology. The Research Ethics Committee of National Taiwan University, Taiwan, approved this study.

Data analysis plan

The aim of the study was to examine the temporal stability of behavior problems in LT children compared to TLD children from toddlerhood to preschool age. First, to understand the relationship between changes in T scores from ages 2 to 4 in the LT and TLD groups and total problems, a group (LT versus TLD) by time (age 2/time 1 versus age 4/time 2) mixed-model ANOVA was performed. To assess the relationship between changes in T scores at both ages in the two groups and internalizing and externalizing behaviors and syndromes, 2 × 2 repeated-measures multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) was performed to adjust for chance significance due to multiple testing. Additionally, sleep problems subscale scores were not included in the internalizing and externalizing scales; rather, a 2 × 2 mixed-model ANOVA was conducted to test the sleep problem syndrome scores. The above tests adopted a significance level (α) of p < .05. Furthermore, each MANOVA was followed by 2 × 2 mixed-model ANOVAs for each dependent variable; where appropriate, degrees of freedom were adjusted using the Greenhouse-Geisser procedure when indicated necessary by Mauchly’s sphericity test. Furthermore, p-values were adjusted according to the Bonferroni procedure to guard against type-1 errors. Therefore, follow-up ANOVAs with internalizing and externalizing behaviors as dependent variables adopted a significance level (α) of p < .025 (=.05/2), those with internalizing syndromes (emotionally reactive, anxious/depressed, somatic complaints, and withdrawn) as dependent variables adopted p < .0125 (=.05/4), and those with externalizing syndromes (attention and aggression) as dependent variables adopted p < .025 (=.05/2). Effect sizes from the ANOVAs and MANOVAs were calculated using partial eta square (ηp2), which can be translated directly into percent of variance explained. Cohen [35] provided a basic framework for interpreting these effects as small (ηp2 = .01), moderate (ηp2 = .06), or large (ηp2 = .15). Due to small sample sizes, p-values less than or equal to 0.10 and ηp2 greater than or equal to .06 were considered to indicate trends. If behavior problems of LT children were more temporally stable than those of TLD children during toddlerhood and preschool age, a significant main effect of group would be found in the above tests.

Next, Fisher’s exact tests were conducted to test the differences in the percentages of participants with behavior problems between the LT and TLD groups for each of the CBCL scales and subscales at ages 2 and 4. Fisher’s exact test was also used to further understand whether the proportion of the presence of behavior problems at ages 2 and age 4 versus only at age 2 differed between LT children and TLD children. These tests were considered significant using a Bonferroni-corrected α level, as in the ANOVAs mentioned above. Due to small sample sizes, p-values less than or equal to 0.10 were considered to indicate trends. Finally, with each CBCL scale and subscale, the association of vocabulary size with concurrent behavior problems (i.e., at age 2) and those that develop over time (i.e., at age 4) among toddlers was examined by logistic regression analyses. All statistical analyses in this study were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 22.0.

Results

Behavior problem T scores: MANOVAs and ANOVAs

Table 3 shows the T scores of the ten CBCL scales and subscales for the LT and TLD groups at ages 2 and 4. The association was assessed between T-score changes from age 2 to 4 in the LT and TLD groups and each of the CBCL scales and subscales.

Table 3 T scores for CBCL scales and subscales in the TLD and LT groups

Main problems (scales)

A mixed-model ANOVA with total problems as the dependent variable revealed a significant main effect of group (F(1, 60) = 4.15, p =.046, ηp2 = .07), but no significant main effect of time, nor a group by time interaction (ps > .05). LT children had more total behavior problems than did TLD children between the ages of 2 and 4. Next, a mixed-model repeated-measures MANOVA was tested with internalizing and externalizing behaviors as dependent variables; there was no significant main effect of group and time, nor a group by time interaction (ps > .05). A follow-up ANOVA with internalizing behaviors as the dependent variable revealed a main effect of time that approached significance (F(1, 60) = 5.14, p =.027, ηp2 = .08); there was no significant main effect of group, nor a group by time interaction (ps > .05). A trend was present toward internalizing behaviors being greater at age 4 than at age 2.

Syndromes (subscales)

A mixed-model repeated-measures MANOVA was assessed with internalizing syndromes (emotionally reactive, anxious/depressed, somatic complaints, and withdrawn) as dependent variables. The model indicated an omnibus group by syndromes interaction that approached significance (Wilks’ lambda = 0.89, F(3, 58) = 2.30, p = .087, ηp2 = .11), but no significant effect of group, nor a group by time interaction (ps > .05). A follow-up ANOVA with emotionally reactive dependent variable revealed a main effect of group that approached significance (F(1, 60) = 4.59, p =.036, ηp2 = .07). LT toddlers exhibited a trend toward higher emotional reactivity than TLD toddlers between the ages of 2 and 4. The other follow-up ANOVA with anxious/depressed syndrome as the dependent variable revealed a significant main effect of time (F(1, 60) = 8.55, p =.005, ηp2 = .13) and a main effect of group that approached significance (F(1, 60) = 4.92, p =.030, ηp2 = .08). Anxiety/depression was higher at age 4 than at age 2, and LT toddlers exhibited a trend toward higher anxiety/depression than TLD toddlers between the ages of 2 and 4. Another mixed-model repeated-measures MANOVA was assessed with externalizing syndromes (attention and aggression) as dependent variables. No significant effects were found of group, nor group by time and group by syndrome interactions (ps > .05). Finally, a mixed-model ANOVA with sleep problems syndrome as the dependent variable revealed no significant effect of group, nor group by time and group by syndrome interactions (ps > .05).

Note that 33 items do not contribute to the seven syndrome subscales and are labeled “other problems” (e.g., “Afraid to try new things,” “Too shy or timid,” “Cries a lot,” and so forth) in the CBCL protocol. These items did not contribute to the internalizing and externalizing scale scores but only contributed to the total problems scale score. The LT children’s other problem scores at age 2 and at age 4 were 13.97 (SD = 5.89) and 12.81 (SD = 6.17), respectively. The TLD children’s other problem scores at age 2 and at age 4 were 10.29 (SD = 4.97) and 9.77 (SD = 5.12), respectively. A mixed-model ANOVA was used to assess how other problem raw scores changed from age 2 to 4 in the LT and TLD groups. A significant main effect of group (F(1, 60) = 8.49, p = .005, ηp2 = .12) was found, but no main effect of time, nor a group by time interaction (ps > .05). For the other problems subscale, LT children exhibited greater behavioral problems than did TLD children between the ages of 2 and 4.

Presence and absence of behavior problems: Fisher’s exact test

Table 4 lists the number of participants for all ten of the CBCL scales and subscales at both time points for the LT and TLD groups. Fisher’s exact test was used to test differences between the LT and TLD groups in the percentages of participants with behavior problems for each of the CBCL scales and subscales at ages 2 and 4. (1) The presence of behavior problems based on total problems (i.e., total problems scale) was greater in the LT group than in the TLD group at age 2 and at age 4 (ps =.037 and .024, respectively). (2) The difference between groups approached significance for internalizing behaviors at age 4 (p = .092). (3) For emotionally reactive and withdrawal behaviors, the difference between groups approached significance (ps =.052 and .056, respectively) at age 2. Moreover, the presence of behavior problems based on withdrawal behaviors was greater in the LT group than in the TLD group at 4 years of age, although the difference fell just outside significance (p = .013). Therefore, a trend toward a higher percentage of emotionally reactive behaviors was found in the LT group versus the TLD group at age 2. Furthermore, at both ages, there were higher percentages of the presence of total behavior problems (that is, the total problems scale) in the LT group than in the TLD group and a trend toward higher percentages of withdrawal behaviors in the LT group than in the TLD group.

Table 4 Presence of behavioral problems changed from time 1 (age 2) to time 2 (age 4) in the TLD and LT groups

Table 4 also presents the percentage of consistent behavior problems in those children with behavior problems at age 2. Fisher’s exact tests showed that among LT and TLD toddlers with the presence of behavior problems at age 2, the LT group had a higher percentage of participants with stable behavior problems than did the TLD group in terms of total problems (p = .004). Overall, early behavior problems were more stable among LT children than among TLD children across very young ages. Furthermore, the percentage of the presence of withdrawal behaviors among LT children increased during toddlerhood and preschool age.

Association between toddler vocabulary size and their behavior problems

Logistic regression revealed that toddlers with larger vocabularies were less likely to exhibit withdrawal behaviors at age 4 (odds ratio [OR] = 0.43, 95% confidence interval [CI] = [0.19, 0.95], p =.037) but not at age 2 (p >.05). Therefore, toddlers with large vocabularies were less likely to develop withdrawal behavior by age 4.

Discussion

Unlike previous large-scale studies [3, 16, 17], this study used a rigorous matched pairs design following two groups of children from ages 2 to 4. This study had high retention rates, and the effect sizes were moderate to large. These factors increased the validity of the findings from this study [3, 16, 17]. Furthermore, this is one of the few longitudinal studies examining the temporal stability of behavior problems in LT toddlers across early childhood. The main findings are discussed below.

Regarding the early language delay being associated with developing behavior problems, according to parental reports, we found that LT children were developing more behavior problems than TLD children from a very young age. Moreover, LT children exhibit a trend to develop internalizing behaviors, such as emotional reactivity and withdrawal, across early years, but not externalizing behaviors. Previous findings indicated that LT children predominately exhibit internalizing behaviors in toddlerhood [15, 19] and at preschool ages [3, 16]. Additionally, other problem syndromes (e.g., afraid to try new things) only contributing to the total problems score were more severe in LT children than in TLD children during toddlerhood and preschool age. Therefore, in addition to focusing on the development of internalizing behaviors among LT toddlers, it is important to address other behavior problems that do not contribute to internalizing and externalizing problems.

Compared with TLD children, LT children tended to be more stable regarding the behavior problems. Previous studies reported only that LT toddlers have behavior problems at preschool ages [3, 16] and school ages [3, 17]. Nonetheless, our findings showed that there was persistence in the behavior problems of LT toddlers in the later developmental stage (e.g., preschool age) associated with the temporal stability of early behavior problems. In other words, LT children exhibit a trend of continuously experiencing more behavior problems than TLD children in emotional reactivity and anxious/depressed behaviors between toddlerhood and preschool age. Furthermore, among LT and TLD toddlers with the presence of behavior problems, the LT group had a higher percentage of participants with stable behavior problems than the TLD group in terms of total problems during preschool age. These results revealed the developmental continuity of early behavior problems through early childhood apparent from toddlerhood among LT children.

The association of language delay with temporal stability of behavior problems was found among LT children from a very young age. Three hypotheses may explain the nature of this association between language delay and behavior problems. For example, language delay and behavior problems may co-occur due to common biological (e.g., neurological injury or damage) or environmental risks (e.g., low parental education level or family income) that create broad developmental vulnerabilities [7, 36]. Alternatively, because language is a social behavior, language difficulties may impede positive interactions and lead to withdrawal and frustration [7]. Behavioral difficulties can interfere with language development by limiting the frequency and quality of interactions, thereby decreasing the exposure to rich linguistic input [36]. Understanding the processes underlying early language delays and persistent behavior problems in LT toddlers warrants further investigation with a larger sample size and a longitudinal design.

Both LT and TLD toddlers showed a common trend for increased internalizing behaviors, such as anxious and depressed behaviors, at age 4. One possible explanation is that parents had higher tolerance of their child’s behavior problems early in toddlerhood; however, they became more sensitive to such behavior problems during the preschool years [15]. Additionally, the percentage of children with increased social withdrawal behaviors was higher among LT toddlers than among TLD toddlers. One possible explanation is that daily learning activities and behavioral regulation demands exceed the language skills of LT children during early childhood. That is, parents may have noticed LT children’s behavior problems in their daily lives because LT children are inefficient at using language to regulate their behaviors. Thus, LT children feel frustrated and exhibit more internalizing behaviors. The mechanisms of change underlying these patterns of behavior problems among LT toddlers from toddlerhood to preschool age are complex and need further evaluation.

The sex ratio in this study was approximately 7:3 (male:female). This was uneven but similar to that of a previous report [17]. This may represent a natural sex distribution of LT toddlers in the community rather than a selection bias. A significant limitation of this study is that it employed the CBCL as the sole measure for categorizing behavior problems. Although the CBCL is an effective and inexpensive instrument for screening children’s behavior problems from a very young age, its results are applicable only to general behavior problems (e.g., aggression, somatic complaints, or emotional reactions). Other measures (i.e., behavioral observation in a standard experience context) could be used to assess the specific internalizing behaviors among LT toddlers. Furthermore, parents were the sole informants regarding their children’s behavior problems. Consequently, we could not assess the behavior problems exhibited by children outside the family context (e.g., in the classroom context) [37]. Future research should, therefore, also include teacher reports.

Finally, our findings imply that clinical professionals should assess the behavior problem profiles of LT toddlers when they are identified in toddlerhood and monitor the developmental patterns of these behaviors from that point. Moreover, toddlers with larger vocabularies were less likely to develop problematic withdrawal behavior in the preschool age. Early intervention is vital for these children due to their increased risk of maintaining and developing more severe behavior problems. Therefore, clinicians working with LT toddlers and their parents should focus not only on language skill promotion but also on behavior problem intervention.

Conclusions

This 2-year prospective case-control study identified an association between early language delay with temporal stability of early behavior problems. A higher percentage of LT toddlers than TLD toddlers had persistent behavior problems. It is also important to note that toddlers with larger vocabularies were less likely to develop withdrawal behaviors over time. Early intervention is essential to decrease the likelihood of presenting behavior problems in later childhood years based on language delays. In addition, it is worth noting that delayed speech is often as frustrating for the child as it is for the parent who struggles to understand what the child is saying. We suggest that LT toddlers at risk of developing behavior problems should be identified and enrolled in early intervention services, which may also increase parents’ understanding of toddler communication and decrease their parenting stress.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

LT:

Late talking

TLD:

Typical language development

References

  1. Hawa VV, Spanoudis G. Toddlers with delayed expressive language: an overview of the characteristics, risk factors and language outcomes. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35:400–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.10.027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Moyle J, Stokes SF, Klee T. Early language delay and specific language impairment. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2011;17:160–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/ddrr.1110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Singleton NC. Late talkers: why the wait-and-see approach is outdated. Pediatr Clin. 2018;65:13–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pcl.2017.08.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Zubrick SR, Taylor CL, Rice ML, Slegers DW. Late language emergence at 24 months: an epidemiological study of prevalence, predictors, and covariates. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2007;50:1562–92. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Rescorla L. Age 17 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers: support for a dimensional perspective on language delay. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2009;52:16–30. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Vallotton C, Ayoub C. Use your words: the role of language in the development of toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Child Res Q. 2011;26:169–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.09.002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Cole PM, Armstrong LM, Pemberton CK. The role of language in the development of emotion regulation. Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition: American Psychological Association; 2010. https://doi.org/10.1037/12059-004.

  8. Humphreys KL, Machlin LS, Guyon-Harris KL, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH. Psychosocial deprivation and receptive language ability: a two-sample study. J Neurodev Disord. 2020;12:36. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-020-09341-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Longobardi E, Spataro P, Frigerio A, Rescorla L. Language and social competence in typically developing children and late talkers between 18 and 35 months of age. Early Child Dev Care. 2016;186:436–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2015.1039529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Manning BL, Roberts MY, Estabrook R, Petitclerc A, Burns JL, Briggs-Gowan M, et al. Relations between toddler expressive language and temper tantrums in a community sample. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2019;65:101070. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2015.1039529.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Roberts MY, Curtis P, Estabrook R, Norton ES, Davis M, Burns J, et al. Talking tots and the terrible twos: early language and disruptive behavior in toddlers. J Dev Behav Pediatr JDBP. 2018;39:709. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000615.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Roulstone S, Law J, Rush R, Clegg J, Peters T. Investigating the role of language in children’s early educational outcomes: UK Department of Education; 2011.

  13. Whitehouse AJ, Robinson M, Zubrick SR. Late talking and the risk for psychosocial problems during childhood and adolescence. Pediatrics. 2011;128:e324–32. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-2782.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Clegg J, Hollis C, Mawhood L, Rutter M. Developmental language disorders–a follow-up in later adult life. Cognitive, language and psychosocial outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:128–49. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00342.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Henrichs J, Rescorla L, Donkersloot C, Schenk JJ, Raat H, Jaddoe VW, et al. Early vocabulary delay and behavioral/emotional problems in early childhood: the generation R study. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2013;56:553–66. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Aro T, Laakso M-L, Määttä S, Tolvanen A, Poikkeus A-M. Associations between toddler-age communication and kindergarten-age self-regulatory skills. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2014;57:1405–17. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-12-0411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Hammer CS, Morgan P, Farkas G, Hillemeier M, Bitetti D, Maczuga S. Late talkers: A population-based study of risk factors and school readiness consequences. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017;60:607–26. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0417.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Bayer JK, Ukoumunne OC, Mathers M, Wake M, Abdi N, Hiscock H. Development of children’s internalising and externalising problems from infancy to five years of age. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2012;46:659–68. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867412450076.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Conway LJ, Levickis PA, Mensah F, McKean C, Smith K, Reilly S. Associations between expressive and receptive language and internalizing and externalizing behaviours in a community-based prospective study of slow-to-talk toddlers. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2017;52:839–53. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Lu H-H, Tsao F-M, Tsai J-D. Behavioral problems of Mandarin-speaking late-talking toddlers and preschool aged children: a prospective case–control study in Taiwan. Medicine (Baltimore). 2020;99. https://doi.org/10.1097/MD.0000000000023341.

  21. Snyder HR, Young JF, Hankin BL. Strong homotypic continuity in common psychopathology-, internalizing-, and externalizing-specific factors over time in adolescents. Clin Psychol Sci. 2017;5:98–110. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702616651076.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Taylor HG, Margevicius S, Schluchter M, Andreias L, Hack M. Persisting behavior problems in extremely low birth weight adolescents. J Dev Behav Pediatr JDBP. 2015;36:178. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Basten M, Tiemeier H, Althoff RR, van de Schoot R, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A, et al. The stability of problem behavior across the preschool years: an empirical approach in the general population. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44:393–404. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-9993-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Hannigan LJ, Walaker N, Waszczuk MA, McAdams TA, Eley TC. Aetiological influences on stability and change in emotional and behavioural problems across development: a systematic review. Psychopathol Rev. 2017;4:52–108. https://doi.org/10.5127/pr.038315.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Liu HM, Tsao FM. The standardization and application of Mandarin-Chinese Communicative Developmental Inventory for infants and toddlers. Chin Ment Health J. 2010;23:503–34. https://doi.org/10.30074/FJMH.201012_23(4).0001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Feldman HM, Dale PS, Campbell TF, Colborn DK, Kurs-Lasky M, Rockette HE, et al. Concurrent and predictive validity of parent reports of child language at ages 2 and 3 years. Child Dev. 2005;76:856–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00882.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Hodges R, Baker E, Munro N, McGregor KK. Responses made by late talkers and typically developing toddlers during speech assessments. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2017;19:587–600. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2016.1221452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hodges R, Munro N, Baker E, McGregor K, Heard R. The monosyllable imitation test for toddlers: influence of stimulus characteristics on imitation, compliance and diagnostic accuracy. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2017;52:30–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. De Los RA, Augenstein TM, Wang M, Thomas SA, Drabick DA, Burgers DE, et al. The validity of the multi-informant approach to assessing child and adolescent mental health. Psychol Bull. 2015;141:858. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Robins DL, Fein D, Barton ML, Green JA. The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers: an initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:131–44. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1010738829569.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development. 3rd ed. Antonio: Harcourt Assessment; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Lin B, Huang Y, Huang G, Hsuan C. Child language disorder scale-revised (preschool version): administration manual. Taipei: Ministry of Education; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Chen I, Huang H, Taoh J. Achenbach system of empirically-based assessment (Mandarin-Chinese version): administration manual. Taipei: Psychology Press; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Chen C, Chen R. Wechsler preschool and primary intelligence scale -fourth edition. (Mandarin-Chinese version): technical manual. Taipei: Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences: Academic; 2013.

  36. Carpenter JL, Drabick DA. Co-occurrence of linguistic and behavioural difficulties in early childhood: a developmental psychopathology perspective. Early Child Dev Care. 2011;181:1021–45. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2010.509795.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Bao P, Jing J, Jin Y, Hu X, Liu B, Hu M. Trajectories and the influencing factors of behavior problems in preschool children: a longitudinal study in Guangzhou, China. BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16:178. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-0864-z.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to the families and children who participated in this study. We also thank all of the research study staff.

Funding

This study was funded with a grant to H. H Lu from the Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan (MOST108-2410-040-010-MY3).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

HHL, JDT, and FMT wrote the first and the revised drafts of the paper. HHL conducted the statistical analyses. HHL and FMT designed this study. HHL, JDT, and FMT provided conceptual guidance and edited the manuscript. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Feng-Ming Tsao.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Research Ethics Committee of National Taiwan University, Taiwan, approved this study. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lu, HH., Tsai, JD. & Tsao, FM. Temporal stability of parent-reported behavior problems in late talkers over 2 years: a prospective case-control study from toddlerhood to preschool age. J Neurodevelop Disord 14, 38 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-022-09445-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-022-09445-x

Keywords