- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Neural oscillatory activity and connectivity in children who stutter during a non-speech motor task
Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders volume 15, Article number: 40 (2023)
Abstract
Background
Neural motor control rests on the dynamic interaction of cortical and subcortical regions, which is reflected in the modulation of oscillatory activity and connectivity in multiple frequency bands. Motor control is thought to be compromised in developmental stuttering, particularly involving circuits in the left hemisphere that support speech, movement initiation, and timing control. However, to date, evidence comes from adult studies, with a limited understanding of motor processes in childhood, closer to the onset of stuttering.
Methods
We investigated the neural control of movement initiation in children who stutter and children who do not stutter by evaluating transient changes in EEG oscillatory activity (power, phase locking to button press) and connectivity (phase synchronization) during a simple button press motor task. We compared temporal changes in these oscillatory dynamics between the left and right hemispheres and between children who stutter and children who do not stutter, using mixed-model analysis of variance.
Results
We found reduced modulation of left hemisphere oscillatory power, phase locking to button press and phase connectivity in children who stutter compared to children who do not stutter, consistent with previous findings of dysfunction within the left sensorimotor circuits. Interhemispheric connectivity was weaker at lower frequencies (delta, theta) and stronger in the beta band in children who stutter than in children who do not stutter.
Conclusions
Taken together, these findings indicate weaker engagement of the contralateral left motor network in children who stutter even during low-demand non-speech tasks, and suggest that the right hemisphere might be recruited to support sensorimotor processing in childhood stuttering. Differences in oscillatory dynamics occurred despite comparable task performance between groups, indicating that an altered balance of cortical activity might be a core aspect of stuttering, observable during normal motor behavior.
Background
Developmental stuttering is a neurodevelopmental disorder that affects the forward flow of speech, characterized by sound repetitions, prolongations, and blocks. Stuttering is a multifactorial disorder that arises from complex interactions between genetics, motor, language, cognitive, and emotion regulation abilities which impact neural development, especially in speech motor control networks [88]. Stuttering affects ~ 5% of preschool-age children and ~ 1% of adults [104] and is often associated with negative impacts on quality of life.
Stuttering has been associated with deficits in motor control, particularly in action initiation and timing [1, 16, 22]. People who stutter typically display longer response time and increased variability when initiating movements in both speech and non-speech (for instance involving finger movements) motor tasks [46, 53, 87, 89, 90, 103]. Task complexity and speed particularly influence performance in people who stutter, such that differences in timing and accuracy between people who stutter and people who do not stutter increase in demanding tasks that involve complex sequences of movements or fine timing control (e.g., [7, 33, 89, 90, 94]), while results have been mixed for simple motor tasks, such as hand claps [44, 67, 94].
These reported differences in speech and non-speech motor behavior could be related to subtle structural and functional differences in the sensorimotor system, including reduced size, activation, and connectivity between the left prefrontal, premotor, motor, and auditory cortices, as well as within the cortico-basal ganglia and cortico-cerebellar loops, and greater size, activation, and connectivity in the right prefrontal and auditory cortices [8, 9, 13, 15, 21]. Left hemispheric structural differences have been consistently reported both in children and adults who stutter, while right hemispheric differences have been typically found in adults. This suggests that the left hemisphere might be the core site of disruption in stuttering, while structural changes in the right hemisphere may develop as a compensatory mechanism over time, following repeated recruitment of these areas to support anomalous left hemisphere neural processes (e.g., [16, 59,60,61]).
Studies of speech motor preparation have also reported altered lateralization in adults who stutter [11, 17, 19, 28, 52, 70, 77, 95, 96] and have, in general, supported the role of the right hemisphere in motor control as compensatory. For instance, Vanhoutte and colleagues (2015, 2016) measured the CNV, an event-related potential thought to reflect motor preparation, during fluent and disfluent speech in adults who stutter and in typical adults. They found that adults who stutter displayed heightened CNV over the right hemisphere during speech motor preparation only in fluent speech, while the CNV preceding stuttered words was comparable to that measured in typical participants. In contrast, a study of speech motor preparation with young children (3–6 years of age; [92] did not find differences in lateralization, although the high recovery rate at that age may have masked a true effect. In sum, while atypical involvement of the right sensorimotor system has been consistently reported in adults who stutter, little is known about how the two hemispheres contribute to motor control in children who stutter, close to the onset of the disorder.
The neural mechanisms underlying motor control, including local neuronal processing and network properties, can be investigated non-invasively by measuring neural oscillations—endogenous periodic fluctuations in the excitability of neuronal populations—using electroencephalography, or EEG [14, 34, 35, 51, 79].
Neural oscillations in the beta range (13–30 Hz) have been particularly implicated in sensorimotor processing and motor control. Endogenous, or spontaneous, beta oscillations are present at rest throughout the sensorimotor system, in particular in the somatosensory, motor, and premotor cortices and the basal ganglia [65, 97]. Changes in beta power, i.e., changes in the intensity of neuronal oscillatory activity in the beta band, are considered the signature of motor preparation and execution: beta power is suppressed during movement planning and execution and enhanced after movement completion (e.g., [65]). Reflecting the lateralization of the motor network, changes in beta power are stronger in the left hemisphere during speech tasks, and in the hemisphere contralateral to the moving hand during unimanual tasks [38]. According to one influential hypothesis, the suppression of beta power before and during motor activity may represent the release of motor inhibition necessary to initiate movement, while the increase in beta power after movement may actively maintain the existing motor state, inhibiting new movement initiation and facilitating the integration of sensory feedback [5, 30]. Research on motor disorders such as Parkinson’s disease supports this interpretation, as difficulties in initiating movement have been associated with enhanced beta power at rest and reduced beta modulation during motor planning [43, 81].
Slower oscillations in the delta (2–4 Hz) and theta (5–7 Hz) bands have also been implicated in motor control, although their power is not consistently modulated in motor actions [69]. During movement, a distributed motor network becomes functionally integrated. The dynamic coordination of the motor network is thought to be orchestrated by the interregional synchronization of slow oscillations in the delta and theta bands, which functionally couples different neuronal populations [4, 74, 105]. In addition, slow oscillations in the delta and theta bands have been shown to phase lock to movement onset, controlling the timing of local neuronal activity [42, 69]. Beta oscillations in different regions of the sensorimotor cortex have also been shown to transiently synchronize during motor tasks, although their role in the functional coupling of the motor network is not fully understood [39, 40].
In stuttering research, speech motor preparation and execution have been investigated in adults primarily by assessing beta power modulation during speech. Although these studies are limited, they generally converge in reporting differences in beta modulation during fluent speech between adults who stutter and adults who do not stutter. However, the direction of the effect (increased or decreased beta modulation) and whether differences involved the left or right hemisphere, varied across studies [45, 55, 56, 73, 77, 84, 85, 91]. These heterogeneous results might partly reflect the heterogeneity of the disorder across adult individuals who stutter, as well as differences in the level of difficulty of the speaking condition and in the linguistic computations that were required to perform the tasks across the different studies.
In contrast to the studies assessing beta power changes, oscillatory connectivity during speech and non-speech motor tasks has not been investigated in stuttering. However, at rest, adults who stutter were reported to exhibit anomalous interhemispheric connectivity in multiple frequency bands compared to adults who do not stutter [47]. Finally, while no study has investigated phase locking to movement onset in stuttering, an MEG study [77] reported evidence of reduced time locking of neuronal activity to speech movements in the right motor cortex of adults who stutter compared to adults who do not stutter.
In sum, studies of oscillatory dynamics indicate atypical patterns of sensorimotor beta rhythm and altered resting-state oscillatory connectivity in adults who stutter compared to adults who do not stutter. However, it is unclear whether differences in neural oscillations during speech or non-speech motor tasks are also present in children who stutter. To date, studies of oscillatory activity in children who stutter have been limited to altered beta oscillatory activity at rest or during a rhythm-listening task [32, 64].
In the current study, we fill this critical gap in the literature by assessing whether spatiotemporal patterns of sensorimotor activity around motor action initiation differ between children who stutter and children who do not stutter (ages 5 to 10 years). To examine whether general differences in sensorimotor activities are present in stuttering (that are not limited to speech), we examined neural oscillations elicited by a non-speech motor task. Furthermore, to promote comparable performance across groups, we chose a simple task, consisting of pressing a button in response to an auditory target, which did not require complex movements or fine timing control. We focus on measures of temporal changes in beta power (reflecting dynamics of cortical sensorimotor activity), delta, theta, and beta phase synchronization within and between the left and right hemispheres (reflecting functional coupling of the cortical sensorimotor network) and intertrial consistency of delta and theta phase locking to movement onset (reflecting trial-by-trial variability in the timing of cortical sensorimotor activity). We compared these measures between left and right sensorimotor cortices and between children who stutter and children who do not stutter.
At the behavioral level, considering the simplicity of the task, we did not expect to see behavioral differences in average accuracy or response time. However, given previous results of heightened variability in the timing of movement initiation in both speech and non-speech motor tasks, we expected to see greater variability in response time in the stuttering group. At the neurophysiological level, based on previous results in adults who stutter and on theoretical accounts of stuttering that suggest that the left hemisphere is the core site of disruption, while the right hemisphere is recruited as a compensatory mechanism, we predicted that groups would differ in measures of sensorimotor oscillations in the left hemisphere (contralateral to the response hand used in this task), but potentially not in the right (ipsilateral) hemisphere. For beta power changes, given the mixed results in adult literature, we did not make predictions on the directions of the effect. For the phase synchronization, we expected to see reduced synchronizations over the left hemisphere, reflecting reduced functional connectivity. For the intertrial consistency of phase locking, we expected to observe reduced consistency over the left hemisphere, reflecting greater neural variability and possibly underlying response time variability.
Methods
Participants
All study procedures were approved by the Human Research Protection Program at Michigan State University. Prior to data collection, all children’s primary caregivers gave written informed consent, and children gave assent to participate in the study.
Participants were 16 children with developmental stuttering (6 males) and 18 typically developing children (12 males), ages 5–10 years. All children were monolingual speakers of English, with no neurological deficit according to parental report. Parents reported most children as White and not Hispanic/Latino (Table 1).
Children were included in the stuttering group based on parental reports and confirmation by a certified Speech-Language-Pathologist. Stuttering severity was measured by the Stuttering Severity Instrument, 4th Edition [72], with scores in the group of children who stutter ranging from very mild to severe (range 6–28; median: 18; interquartile range 13–21). Three children were categorized as very mild (SSI-4 score ≤ 10), 7 as mild (SSI-4 score from 11 to 20), 5 as moderate (SSI-4 score from 21 to 27), and 1 as severe stuttering (SSI-4 score = 28).
All participants completed a battery of behavioral speech-language assessments (Table 1). These included measures of non-verbal intelligence (either the Primary Test of Non-verbal Intelligence, PTONI, [29], or the Test of Non-verbal Intelligence, TONI-4, [12], based on age at the time of testing), and receptive and expressive language (Clinical Evaluations of Language Fundamentals Preschool-2, second edition [101], or Clinical Evaluations of Language Fundamentals, Fifth Edition [102] depending on age). All children scored within or above normal range, except one child who does not stutter who had lower performance on the PTONI (standard score: 64). Importantly, the two groups did not differ in these measures (nonverbal intelligence: t(32) = − 1.08, p = .29; language: t(32) = .27, p = .79). All children completed a modified version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory test [63]. All children were right-handed except for two ambidextrous children (one per group) and one left-handed child in the group of children who do not stutter. The groups did not differ in socioeconomic status (SES).Footnote 1
Task and stimuli
All children performed an auditory oddball task, in which they listened to a stream of frequent (standard, 1 kHz) and rare (target, 2 kHz) pure tones pseudo-randomly presented such that target tones were always preceded and followed by at least one standard tone (20% of tones were targets, [48]. Children responded to the rare target tones by pressing a single button on a response pad with their right thumb (Current Design non-MR trainer response pad). Each tone was 50 ms long, including a linear rise and fall time of 5 ms. Stimuli were presented via over-the-ear headphones at a comfortable loudness (~ 75 dB SPL). Inter-stimulus intervals were 200 ms or 1000 ms, pseudorandomly presented such that rare target tones were always followed by the longer, 1000 ms interval to reduce overlap between motor responses and the subsequent tone stimulus. The task was administered in experimental blocks of 300 tones (240 standard and 60 targets per block). Children completed two blocks separated by a short break, except for 3 children in the non-stuttering group and 2 children in the stuttering group, who only completed one block. Brief rest breaks were available every 100 trials as needed. Experimental blocks were preceded by a short practice block (20 tones) to ensure children understood and were able to successfully complete the task. The practice block was repeated until a child could successfully perform the task (maximum practice blocks needed was 3). The groups did not differ in the number of practice or experimental blocks completed (experimental blocks, mean ± SE for both groups 1.8 ± 0.1, t(32) = − 0.08, p = 0.94; practice blocks, children who stutter 0.9 ± 0.2, children who do not stutter 1 ± 0.2, t(32) = 0.24, p = 0.81). The total duration of the task was between 15 and 20 min. The task was the last of a battery of three. Overall, the EEG recording sessions (including EEG setup, tasks explanation, familiarization, and execution, as well as breaks) lasted approximately 1.5 h.
Behavioral measures
For each participant, the hit rate and false alarm rate were computed as the percentage of correct responses after a target tone (hit rate) and after a standard tone (false alarm rate). Response time was defined as the delay between the target onset and the button press on correct trials. For each participant, average response time and variability (standard deviation) of response times were computed. These measures of behavioral performance were compared across the two groups using t-tests.
EEG recording and analyses
EEG data were continuously recorded at 512 Hz using 32 Ag/Ag-Cl electrodes embedded in an elastic cap (Biosemi Active-Two system), with locations consistent with the International 10–20 system. Additional electrodes were placed over the left and right mastoids, left and right outer canthi, and left orbital ridge.
EEG analyses were performed using EEGLAB v2021.1 [26] and custom made routines in MATLAB R2016b (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). EEG recordings were referenced offline to the average of the left and right mastoids, filtered between 1 and 60 Hz and down-sampled to 256 Hz. The portions of recordings collected during breaks were removed, leaving 2-s buffer zones for edge artifacts. The remaining EEG data were inspected visually and with the aid of clean_rawdata EEGLAB plugin (https://sccn.ucsd.edu/wiki/EEGLAB_Extensions). Channels were removed if they had poor correlations with neighboring channels (below 0.85) or contained non-transient line noise greater than 4 standard deviations beyond the channel population mean. On average, 1.9 channels were removed per subject (most common: T7 and T8, over the temporal region). Time windows contaminated with artifacts were identified with artifact subspace reconstruction [49], with a rejection threshold of 20. ASR has been shown to improve the quality of ICA decomposition [2, 18]. Oculomotor, muscle, and channel artifacts were identified and subtracted from the data via Independent Component Analysis (ICA) in the EEGLAB toolbox. On average 2.3 components were removed after visual inspection of their topography, time course, and power spectrum.
Only trials with correct motor responses were analyzed. For each subject, EEG data were segmented into 3-s epochs, from 1 s before to 2 s after the onset of correct button presses (epochs were 3 s to allow space for edge artifacts induced by wavelet convolution). We analyzed the data within three frequency bands: delta (2–4 Hz), theta (5–7 Hz), and beta (13–30 Hz). Analyses were restricted to bilateral electrodes over the premotor and sensorimotor cortical areas (i.e., the frontocentral electrodes: F3, F4, FC5, FC6, C3, C4, C5, C6, Fig. 1A) and to the time interval − 200 to 200 ms around the button press, which comprises a preparatory period (− 200 to 0 ms) and an execution period (0 to 200 ms). Due to the fast pace of the task, no stationary baseline (i.e., periods with no movement-related EEG signals) was present in the task, thus all analyses focused on the comparison between the execution and the preparatory periods.
Spectrotemporal analyses
Time-frequency decomposition was performed via complex Morlet wavelet convolution in the frequency range of 2–50 Hz (in 40 steps). The number of cycles in the wavelets increased linearly from 3 to 10. Figure 1 shows the three measures of oscillatory dynamics.
Power was computed from the wavelet transform at each frequency and time point. For each participant and each electrode, power changes around the button press were computed as the difference in decibels between the average power in the 200 ms after the button press minus the average power in the 200 ms before the button press (Fig. 1C). To visualize the time course of beta power (Fig. 3A), power was baselined corrected with a baseline from − 600 to + 600 ms from button press.
Functional connectivity between neural populations was measured as the phase synchronization between same-band oscillatory activity at two electrodes. Multiple indices have been proposed to measure phase synchronization [6]. Here we chose the weighted phase-lag index (wPLI) because it is robust to volume conduction effects, that is, it does not yield false connectivity due to shared sources [6, 24, 99]. The wPLI measures the distribution of phase differences between two EEG signals across trials. To eliminate volume conduction effects, the wPLI disregards instantaneous coupling (i.e., it disregards phase angle differences equal to zero degrees) and weights phase angle differences farther from zero progressively more. The wPLI values range between 0 and 1, with 0 indicating no connectivity (i.e., a random distribution of phase differences over trials) and 1 indicating true lagged connectivity (i.e., the same non-zero phase difference in all trials). In particular, we measured the debiased weighted phase-lag index (dwPLI), which is an adjusted version of the wPLI index that is more robust to bias due to small sample size, as well as volume conduction and noise [6, 24, 99]. To evaluate changes in phase synchronization around the button press response, we computed the difference between the average dwPLI in the 200 ms after the button press minus the average dwPLI in the 200 ms before the button press for each frequency band (Fig. 1B, D).
Phase locking was computed using the inter-trial phase clustering index (ITPC), which measures the consistency of an oscillation phase angle at a given time across trials [24]. ITPC values range between 0 and 1, with 0 indicating high variability of phases across trials, and 1 indicating no variability, i.e., all trials have the same phase. Changes in phase locking around the motor response were computed for each frequency band, as the difference between the average ITPC in the 200 ms after the button press minus the average ITPC in the 200 ms before the button press (Fig. 1E).
Statistical analysis
For each dependent variable (power, phase synchronization, and phase locking) in different frequency bands, we performed a mixed-model ANOVA using a linear mixed-effects model [58]. For the analysis of power and phase locking, fixed effects were Group (stuttering and non-stuttering) and hemisphere (left and right). For the analysis of phase synchronization, fixed effects were group (stuttering and non-stuttering) and location (left hemisphere, right hemisphere, between-hemispheres). The subject was modeled as a random intercept. Post-hoc comparisons were performed when the interaction and main effects were significant. Post-hoc comparisons were computed with F-tests on fixed-effects coefficients of the linear mixed-effects models using appropriate contrasts. p-values were adjusted for multiple comparisons with the Benjamini and Hochberg correction [10].
To investigate the relation between stuttering severity and oscillatory measures, we computed Pearson correlations between SSI-4 scores and average power, phase locking, and phase synchronization in each hemisphere and between hemispheres for phase synchronization.
Results
Behavioral performance was comparable in the two groups
Children who stutter and children who do not stutter exhibited comparable task performance in terms of hit rate, false alarms, average response time, and variability of the response time, as shown in Fig. 2 and Table 2.
Weaker beta power modulation over the left hemisphere in children who stutter
Figure 3A shows the average beta power over the left and right frontocentral regions (see EEG electrodes location in Fig. 1) for the two groups. Children who do not stutter exhibited a brief beta power increase over the left hemisphere following the button press response, which was not evident in children who stutter.
We quantified beta power change around button press as the difference between the average beta power in the 200 ms window immediately after the button press and the average beta power in the 200 ms window immediately before the button press (time windows are indicated in yellow in Fig. 3A). A mixed-model ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between group and hemisphere, indicating that power changes around button press were left-lateralized in the non-stuttering group, but not in the stuttering group (F(1,268) = 4.34; p = .038, Fig. 3B). In post-hoc analysis, the difference between the two groups in left hemisphere beta power changes did not survive adjustment for multiple comparisons (F(1,268) = 4.45, non-adjusted p = .036, adjusted p = .072), and the comparison of beta power changes between hemispheres revealed left lateralization only in children who do not stutter (F(1,268) = 9.45; adjusted p = 0.009). We also saw a significant main effect of hemisphere (F(1,268) = 4.56; p = .034), which was driven by the non-stuttering group. The main effect of group was not significant (F(1,268) = 2.32; p = .13).
Oscillatory synchronization was weaker at low frequencies and less left lateralized in the beta band in children who stutter
To assess functional coupling within and between hemispheres, we analyzed phase synchronization in the delta, theta, and beta bands. The time course of average phase synchronization over the left and right hemispheres and between hemispheres is shown in Fig. 4A for each group. At low frequencies, in the delta and theta bands, phase synchronization increased soon after the button press in the non-stuttering group more than in the stuttering group. In contrast, in the beta band, the magnitude of phase synchronization was overall comparable between the two groups, but the spatial pattern was different, with greater synchronization between hemispheres in the stuttering group and greater synchronization over the left hemisphere in the non-stuttering group.
Changes in phase synchronization around button press response (200 ms window after button press minus 200 ms window before button press) were analyzed with mixed-model ANOVAs in each band (Fig. 4B, Table 3). In the delta and theta bands, we found a main effect of group, confirming that phase synchronization increased in the non-stuttering more than in the stuttering group (delta: F(1,946) = 9.84; p = .002, Theta: F(1,946) = 4.81; p = .029). In the theta band, we also observed a significant interaction between group and location (F(2,946) = 4.15; p = .016). Post-hoc comparisons showed that the difference between non-stuttering and stuttering groups was larger over the left hemisphere (p = .01; Table 4).
In the beta band, we found a significant interaction between group and location, such that synchronization was left-lateralized in the non-stuttering, but not in the stuttering group (F(2,946) = 7.53; p < .001). Post-hoc comparisons (Table 4) revealed stronger left hemisphere synchronization compared to synchronization over the right hemisphere or between hemispheres in children who do not stutter. In contrast, in children who stutter, left hemisphere and interhemispheric synchronization were comparable, and interhemispheric synchronization was stronger than right hemisphere synchronization. No significant differences in synchronization were observed between the two groups over left, right, or between hemispheres, but interhemispheric synchronization was marginally greater in children who stutter compared to children who do not stutter (Table 4).
Delta, but not theta, phase locking was less left-lateralized in the children who stutter
To assess the cross-trial variability in oscillatory dynamics, we analyzed changes in the slow oscillatory phase locking to the button press. Figure 5A shows the time course of the average delta and theta phase locking in the left and right frontocentral regions. In both groups, phase locking to button press increases starting a few hundred milliseconds before the button press. Changes in phase locking (200 ms after minus 200 ms before the button press) were analyzed with a mixed-model ANOVA in each band. In the delta band, there was a significant interaction between group and hemisphere (F(1,268) = 5.40; p = .021), indicating that the change in delta phase locking was left lateralized only in children who do not stutter. A significant effect of hemisphere (F(1,268) = 8.85; p = .003) was also observed, driven by the hemisphere differences in the children who do not stutter, as post-hoc analysis revealed left-lateralized delta phase locking only in the non-stuttering group (adjusted p value of 0.006). In the theta band, there was a main effect of hemisphere (F(1,268) = 9.08; p = .0028) indicating that theta phase locking was left-lateralized in both groups (Fig. 5B).
Correlation between stuttering severity and measures of oscillatory dynamics
No correlation between stuttering severity (as measured by SSI-4 scores) and oscillatory measures survived multiple comparisons, but uncorrected correlation analysis suggested a negative relation between stuttering severity and the average delta phase synchronization over the right hemisphere, such that children with more severe stuttering tended to exhibit reduced phase synchronization in the delta band over the right hemisphere (r = – 0.53, non-adjusted p = 0.03, adjusted p > 0.05).
Discussion
We investigated the spatiotemporal dynamics of sensorimotor oscillatory activity and connectivity around movement initiation in children who stutter and children who do not stutter. Children pressed a button in response to rare auditory targets embedded in a stream of non-target tones. Although task performance in terms of speed and accuracy was comparable between the two groups, the spatiotemporal dynamics of oscillatory activity and network connectivity during the non-speech movement differed. This suggests that differences in general sensorimotor control (non-speech) are present in children who stutter and that the examination of oscillatory dynamics is a sensitive tool to investigate them.
Behavioral performance
Given the simplicity of the task and required motor movement (responses consisted of a simple one-finger button press), we expected to find comparable behavioral accuracy and response speed, but we speculated that children who stutter might be more variable in response timing across trials. Indeed, consistent with our expectation and with previous studies that employed simple motor tasks [44, 67], we found comparable accuracy and mean response time between the two groups. Children who stutter tended to be slightly more variable in their response time, but the difference did not reach significance (p = .07). This is consistent with previous reports of higher variability in the timing of movement initiation in both speech and non-speech motor tasks, which may result in significant group differences when performing more challenging tasks [33, 87, 103].
Beta power modulation
In this study, we found a lack of left-lateralized peri-motor beta power modulation in children who stutter. After the button press, beta power increased over the left hemisphere for children who do not stutter, while it slightly decreased over both hemispheres in children who stutter. This result is in general agreement with previous studies of oscillatory activity in adults who stutter, which have implicated altered modulation and lateralization of beta oscillations in the preparation and execution of fluent speech [45, 55, 56, 73, 77, 84, 85, 91]. However, previous studies reported variation in the direction of the effect, with both stronger and weaker modulation of beta power being reported (e.g., [45, 56]). In part, the inconsistencies in the results of previous studies with adults who stutter may reflect the heterogeneity of the disorder across individuals, including compensatory mechanisms, which may develop in adults who stutter either spontaneously or through therapy. In contrast, we have investigated children between 5 and 10 years of age, thus closer to the onset of the disorder.
Our results of weaker modulation of left peri-motor beta in the stuttering group are similar to results by Jenson and colleagues [45] for speech production in adults who stutter, although methodological differences, including the nature and timing of the tasks (slow speech production vs. fast finger movement, adults vs. children) preclude direct comparison. The reduced beta modulation found by Jensen and colleagues [45] consisted of reduced beta suppression before and during speech movements over the left hemisphere, which was interpreted as reflecting reduced activation of the forward model of speech; however, the post-movement beta rebound was not evaluated. In contrast, in our investigation, the reduced beta modulation in children who stutter was driven by a weaker rebound of beta activity after button press (see the time course of the beta power in Fig. 3), although we cannot fully exclude that a difference was also present before movement (given the lack of a resting condition, it was not possible to independently estimate the levels of pre-movement suppression and subsequent rebound of beta activity). Indeed, studies of repetitive movements have shown that beta modulation comprises both a sustained suppression lasting the entire movement period, as well as amplitude modulations around each individual movement (e.g., [82]).
The role of post-movement beta rebound is not fully understood, but studies suggest that its modulation is to some extent independent of pre-movement beta decreases (e.g., [41]). According to two competing, but not mutually exclusive hypotheses, the beta rebound may reflect the active inhibition of the motor cortex to suppress further movement [66, 76, 80] and/or it may reflect the post-movement processing of sensory feedback to evaluate movement outcome (e.g., [78]). Furthermore, an EEG study that manipulated task and sensory uncertainty found that the amplitude of beta rebound decreased with increasing uncertainty in feedforward predictions [93]). Thus, reduced beta rebound is consistent with theories on stuttering that suggest unstable internal models and/or higher reliance on sensory feedback in people who stutter compared to people who do not stutter (e.g., [23, 54]). Future studies are necessary to distinguish between these hypotheses.
Oscillatory synchronization
The second main difference in oscillatory dynamics concerns peri-movement changes in oscillatory phase synchronization, a measure of the dynamic functional organization of the sensorimotor network [31, 34, 97, 98]. Consistent with previous studies in unimanual motor tasks [62, 71, 74], typically developing children displayed a widespread increase in slow frequency synchronization (delta/theta) and a left-lateralized pattern of beta synchronization. In the stuttering group, we expected to see less phase synchronization increase over the left hemisphere compared to the non-stuttering group, reflecting reduced functional connectivity within the left sensorimotor network during movement planning and execution. We found a complex pattern of results that partially agreed with our predictions. Relative to children who do not stutter, children who stutter displayed a smaller peri-movement increase in connectivity at slow frequencies (delta/theta), but this difference was widespread, over the left and right hemispheres as well as between hemispheres. In the beta band, children who stutter displayed less left-lateralized connectivity than children who do not stutter, but the difference was driven by an increase in interhemispheric synchronization rather than by a decrease in left hemisphere synchronization.
Phase synchronization in delta/theta bands has been linked to long-distance communication in the bilateral sensorimotor network [74, 97]. Network coupling has been proposed to rely on the interaction of two oscillatory mechanisms: long-range, low frequency phase synchronization and local hierarchical coupling between slow oscillations and fast oscillations/neural spiking. Within the cortical striatal network, invasive studies in animal models [50, 100, 106] support this proposed mechanism, particularly involving theta phase synchronization and theta-gamma coupling, while evidence for delta phase synchronization and delta-beta/gamma coupling is sparser [25, 57, 100]. Human non-invasive studies have related changes in delta/theta interregional synchronization with different phases of movements, such as preparation and execution [74, 75, 105], and have highlighted the role of theta-gamma and theta-beta coupling in sensory feedback processing during speech production [83, 85]. In addition, a computational model of the cortical striatal network has proposed that theta oscillations (with nested gamma oscillations) may encode motor sequences, similar to the encoding of spatial information in the hippocampus [36]. Thus, reduced delta and theta synchronization during movement in children who stutter may indicate less efficient communication in the motor network, which could particularly affect motor planning in complex sequences, such as speech.
The smaller increase in global delta/theta synchronization was accompanied by a larger interhemispheric beta synchronization increase in children who stutter. Interhemispheric beta synchronization has been shown to transiently change with movement initiation, increasing during coordinated bimanual tasks and decreasing during unimanual tasks [27, 62, 86, 97]. The impact of fast and slow oscillatory connectivity on motor control is not fully understood, but the increased interhemispheric beta synchronization suggests the possible recruitment of the right hemisphere in children who stutter during a unimanual simple task performance. This result was unexpected as anomalous lateralization of motor function, which has been reported in adults who stutter both in speech production tasks and a finger tapping task [60, 61], is generally thought to develop in time as a compensatory mechanism [16]. Children in this study were between 5 and 10 years of age, so they had been stuttering for several years at the time of testing (years since stuttering onset: average = 4.2 ± .4, range from 1.5 to 6.9 years, according to parental reports of the age of stuttering onset). Consistently, a study of neuroanatomical connectivity has reported anomalous white matter fractional anisotropy in the corpus callosum and right inferior frontal gyrus in children who stutter between 3 and 10 years of age [20]. More studies with younger children are necessary to understand the role of the right hemisphere in motor control in developmental stuttering.
To our knowledge, no other study has examined changes in within-frequency, long-range phase synchronization during motor tasks in stuttering. At rest, high-frequency interhemispheric synchronization has been reported to be smaller in adults who stutter than in adults who do not stutter, and within the group of stuttering adults, more severe stuttering was associated with stronger interhemispheric low-frequency synchronization [47]. Methodological differences (adults vs. children and resting-state vs. task-evoked connectivity) preclude a direct comparison between our results and those by Joos et al. [47]; however, both studies point to altered network organization in stuttering that merits further investigation.
Low frequency phase locking
We predicted that children who stutter would exhibit greater trial-by-trial variability in slow oscillatory dynamics over the left hemisphere, as indexed by a weaker transient phase locking to button press in the delta and theta bands. Results in the delta band but not in the theta band followed our prediction. In children who do not stutter, the transient increase in delta and theta phase locking to the impending button press was stronger over the left hemisphere (contralateral to the moving hand). These patterns are consistent with previous studies of finger movements in children and adults [3, 37, 69]. In children who stutter, phase locking transiently increased with the same dynamics in both delta and theta bands, but in the delta band, it was not left lateralized; increases were similar across both hemipsheres. Although, as already noted, children who stutter were only marginally more variable in their response times than children who do not stutter, the observed weaker phase locking over the left hemisphere may reflect greater variability in the timing of motor activity.
Limitations
This study has several limitations. The first is the number of participants. Developmental stuttering is a disorder with a complex etiology that involves the interaction of multiple factors, hence even at the onset of the disorder, children who stutter might differ in the neural processing supporting motor behavior. A larger number of participants is necessary to replicate and expand the results reported here. A second limitation of this study is that we did not collect data on the button press movement itself. We cannot rule out that different oscillatory dynamics between groups might have resulted from differences in movement parameters, such as velocity, duration of the press, and force applied to the button, which we did not measure, as we focused on changes in oscillatory measures around movement onset. However, it is possible that these aspects of movement may contribute to the dynamics of oscillatory activity in complex ways. In addition, classical studies of sensorimotor oscillations involved isolated movements or slow sequences of movements which allow the independent estimate of suppression and rebound of beta activity relative to a resting level [65]. In contrast, we characterized oscillatory dynamics in a fast-paced stream of target and non-target sounds with short inter-sound-intervals. While future studies with longer windows around each movement might uncover the full dynamics of movement-related oscillatory activity, we show that different oscillatory dynamics at movement initiation can be detected in children who stutter even in ecologically relevant fast-paced tasks.
Finally, it is important to note that the oscillatory activity in our task might reflect sensorimotor as well as other cognitive processing, such as memory, attention, and effort [97]. Although these cognitive operations cannot be fully disambiguated, our results suggest that children who stutter and typically developing children differ in oscillatory dynamics around movement initiation, and in particular in the balance between the different frequency bands and the two hemispheres. Future studies involving diverse tasks and multiple methodologies may clarify the contribution of cognitive processes to these differences.
Conclusions
The onset of movement is reliably accompanied by changes in oscillatory activity in the motor cortex, particularly in the hemisphere contralateral to the moving hand. In this study, we identified several differences in oscillatory dynamics between children who stutter and children who do not stutter, indexed by reduced modulation of power and phase synchronization over the left hemisphere, reduced lateralization of phase locking to movement initiation at slow frequencies, and altered interhemispheric phase synchronization in the delta and beta bands. Taken together, these results point to deficits in the functioning of the left sensorimotor cortex in stuttering that may not be limited to speech and suggest that the right hemisphere may be recruited to support sensorimotor processing even in childhood developmental stuttering.
Availability of data and materials
The data described in this research are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Notes
SES was measured by assessing maternal and paternal education as well as household income in the previous year. Maternal and paternal education was coded on a 1 to 18 scale based on years of education as follows: 10Â =Â some high school; 12Â =Â completed high school; 13Â =Â partial college; 14Â =Â 2-year college degree; 16Â =Â standard college/bachelor degree; and 18Â =Â graduate school. Household income was coded on a 1 to 11 scale based on the total income in the previous year as follows: 1Â =Â less than 10,000 $; 2Â =Â 10,000 to 25,000 $; 3Â =Â 25,000 to 40,000 $; 4Â =Â 40,000 to 55,000 $; 5Â =Â 55,000 to 70,000 $; 6Â =Â 70,000 to 85,000 $; 7Â =Â 85,000 to 95,000 $; 8Â =Â 95,000 to 105,000 $; 9Â =Â 105,000 to 150,000 $; 10Â =Â 150,000 to 250,000 $; 11Â =Â more than 250,000 $ (e.g., [68]).
Abbreviations
- CNS:
-
Children who do not stutter
- CWS:
-
Children who stutter
- dwPLI:
-
De-biased weighted phase-lag index
- ITPC:
-
Inter-trial phase clustering
References
Alm P. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disorders. 2004;37:325–69.
Anders P, Müller H, Skjæret-Maroni N, Vereijken B, Baumeister J. The influence of motor tasks and cut-off parameter selection on artifact subspace reconstruction in EEG recordings. Med Biol Eng Comput. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11517-020-02252-3.
Armstrong S, Sale MV, Cunnington R. Neural oscillations and the initiation of voluntary movement. Front Psychol. 2018. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02509.
Babiloni C, Del Percio C, Lopez S, Di Gennaro G, Quarato PP, Pavone L, Morace R, Soricelli A, Noce G, Esposito V, Gallese V, Mirabella G. Frontal functional connectivity of electrocorticographic delta and theta rhythms during action execution versus action observation in humans. Front Behav Neurosci. 2017. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00020.
Baker SN. Oscillatory interactions between sensorimotor cortex and the periphery. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.01.007.
Bastos AM, Schoffelen JM. A tutorial review of functional connectivity analysis methods and their interpretational pitfalls. Front Syst Neurosci. 2016. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00175.
Bauerly KR, De Nil LF. Speech sequence skill learning in adults who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2011.05.002.
Belyk M, Kraft SJ, Brown S. Stuttering as a trait or state - an ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Eur J Neurosci. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12765.
Belyk M, Kraft SJ, Brown S. Stuttering as a trait or a state revisited: motor system involvement in persistent developmental stuttering. Eur J Neurosci. 2017;45(4):622–4. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13512.Erratumfor:EurJNeurosci.2015Jan;41(2):275-84. (PMID: 28191730).
Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple hypothesis testing. J R Stat Soc B. 1995;57:289–300.
Biermann-Ruben K, Salmelin R, Schnitzler A. Right rolandic activation during speech perception in stutterers: a MEG study. Neuroimage. 2005;25(3):793–801. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.024. (PMID: 15808980).
Brown L, Sherbenou RJ, Johnsen SK. Test of nonverbal intelligence: TONI-4. Pro-ed: Austin, TX; 2010
Brown S, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Laird AR, Fox PT. Stuttered and fluent speech production: an ALE meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):105–17.
Buzsáki G, Draguhn A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1099745.
Chang SE, Garnett EO, Etchell A, Chow HM. Functional and neuroanatomical bases of developmental stuttering: current insights. Neuroscientist. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858418803594.
Chang SE, Guenther FH. Involvement of the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamocortical loop in developmental stuttering. Front Psychol. 2020. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03088.
Chang SE, Horwitz B, Ostuni J, Reynolds R, Ludlow CL. Evidence of left inferior frontal-premotor structural and functional connectivity deficits in adults who stutter. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(11):2507–18. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr028 Epub 2011 Apr 6. PMID: 21471556; PMCID: PMC3183422.
Chang CY, Hsu SH, Pion-Tonachini L, Jung TP. Evaluation of artifact subspace reconstruction for automatic artifact components removal in multi-channel EEG recordings. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2019.2930186.
Chang SE, Kenney MK, Loucks TM, Ludlow CL. Brain activation abnormalities during speech and non-speech in stuttering speakers. Neuroimage. 2009;46(1):201–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.066 Epub 2009 Feb 11. PMID: 19401143; PMCID: PMC2693291.
Chang SE, Zhu DC, Choo AL, Angstadt M. White matter neuroanatomical differences in young children who stutter. Brain. 2015;138(Pt 3):694–711. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awu400.
Chow HM, Chang SE. White matter developmental trajectories associated with persistence and recovery of childhood stuttering. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23590.
Civier O, Bullock D, Max L, Guenther FH. Computational modeling of stuttering caused by impairments in a basal ganglia thalamo-cortical circuit involved in syllable selection and initiation. Brain Lang. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.016.
Civier O, Tasko SM, Guenther FH. Overreliance on auditory feedback may lead to sound/syllable repetitions: simulations of stuttering and fluency-inducing conditions with a neural model of speech production. J Fluency Disord. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2010.05.002.
Cohen MX. Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2014.
Dejean C, Arbuthnott G, Wickens JR, Le Moine C, Boraud T, Hyland BI. Power fluctuations in beta and gamma frequencies in rat globus pallidus: association with specific phases of slow oscillations and differential modulation by dopamine D1 and D2 receptors. J Neurosci. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3311-09.2011.
Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009.
de Oliveira SC, Gribova A, Donchin O, Bergman H, Vaadia E. Neural interactions between motor cortical hemispheres during bimanual and unimanual arm movements. Eur J Neurosci. 2001. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01801.
De Nil LF, Kroll RM, Lafaille SJ, Houle S. A positron emission tomography study of short- and long-term treatment effects on functional brain activation in adults who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2003 Winter;28(4):357-79; quiz 379-80.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2003.07.002. PMID: 14643070.
Ehrler D, McGhee R. Primary Test of Nonverbal Intelligence. Pro-Ed. 2008
Engel AK, Fries P. Beta-band oscillations–signalling the status quo? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20(2):156–65.
Engel AK, Singer W. Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01568-0.
Etchell AC, Ryan M, Martin E, Johnson BW, Sowman PF. Abnormal time course of low beta modulation in non-fluent preschool children: a magnetoencephalographic study of rhythm tracking. Neuroimage. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.086.
Falk S, Müller T, Bella SD. Sensorimotor Synchronization in Stuttering Children and Adolescents. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2014;126:206–7.
Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011.
Fries P. Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034.
Fukai T. Sequence generation in arbitrary temporal patterns from theta-nested gamma oscillations: a model of the basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loops. Neural Netw. 1999. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0893-6080(99)00057-x.
Gaetz W, Macdonald M, Cheyne D, Snead OC. Neuromagnetic imaging of movement-related cortical oscillations in children and adults: age predicts post-movement beta rebound. Neuroimage. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.077.
Gehrig J, Wibral M, Arnold C, Kell CA. Setting up the speech production network: how oscillations contribute to lateralized information routing. Front Psychol. 2012. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00169.
Gerloff C, Corwell B, Chen R, Hallett M, Cohen LG. The role of the human motor cortex in the control of complex and simple finger movement sequences. Brain. 1998. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.9.1695.
Gross J, Pollok B, Dirks M, Timmermann L, Butz M, Schnitzler A. Task-dependent oscillations during unimanual and bimanual movements in the human primary motor cortex and SMA studied with magnetoencephalography. Neuroimage. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.025.
Hall SD, Stanford IM, Yamawaki N, McAllister CJ, Rönnqvist KC, Woodhall GL, Furlong PL. The role of GABAergic modulation in motor function related neuronal network activity. NeuroImage. 2011;56(3):1506–10.
Hamel-Thibault A, Thénault F, Whittingstall K, Bernier PM. Delta-band oscillations in motor regions predict hand selection for reaching. Cerebral Cortex. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw392.
Heinrichs-Graham E, Wilson TW, Santamaria PM, Heithoff SK, Torres-Russotto D, Hutter-Saunders JA, Estes KA, Meza JL, Mosley RL, Gendelman HE. Neuromagnetic evidence of abnormal movement-related beta desynchronization in Parkinson’s disease. Cerebral cortex. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht121.
Hilger AI, Zelaznik H, Smith A. Evidence that bimanual motor timing performance is not a significant factor in developmental stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-S-15-0172.
Jenson D, Reilly KJ, Harkrider AW, Thornton D, Saltuklaroglu T. Trait related sensorimotor deficits in people who stutter: an EEG investigation of μ rhythm dynamics during spontaneous fluency. Neuroimage Clin. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2018.05.026.
Jones RD, White AJ, Lawson KH, Anderson TJ. Visuoperceptual and visuomotor deficits in developmental stutterers: an exploratory study. Hum Mov Sci. 2002;21(5–6):603–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-9457(02)00165-3. (PMID: 12620714).
Joos K, De Ridder D, Boey RA, Vanneste S. Functional connectivity changes in adults with developmental stuttering: a preliminary study using quantitative electro-encephalography. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00783.
Kaganovich N, Wray AH, Weber-Fox C. Non-linguistic auditory processing and working memory update in pre-school children who stutter: an electrophysiological study. Dev Neuropsychol. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.508549.
Kothe CAE, Jung TP. Artifact removal techniques with signal reconstruction, U.S. Patent Application No. 14/895,440, 2014
Igarashi J, Isomura Y, Arai K, Harukuni R, Fukai T. A θ–γ oscillation code for neuronal coordination during motor behavior. J Neuroscience. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2126-13.2013.
Lakatos P, Karmos G, Mehta AD, Ulbert I, Schroeder CE. Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science. 2008. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1154735.
Lu C, Chen C, Ning N, Ding G, Guo T, Peng D, Yang Y, Li K, Lin C. The neural substrates for atypical planning and execution of word production in stuttering. Exp Neurol. 2010;221(1):146–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.expneurol.2009.10.016. (Epub 2009 Oct 29 PMID: 19879262).
MacPherson MK, Smith A. Influences of sentence length and syntactic complexity on the speech motor control of children who stutter. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research. 2013; https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0184).
Max L, Guenther FH, Gracco VL, Ghosh SS, Wallace ME. Unstable or insufficiently activated internal models and feedback-biased motor control as sources of dysfluency: a theoretical model of stuttering. Contemp Issues Commun Sci Disord. 2004;31:105–22.
Mersov A, Cheyne D, Jobst C, De Nil L. A preliminary study on the neural oscillatory characteristics of motor preparation prior to dysfluent and fluent utterances in adults who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2017.05.003.28577876.
Mersov AM, Jobst C, Cheyne DO, De Nil L. Sensorimotor oscillations prior to speech onset reflect altered motor networks in adults who stutter. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00443.
Morillon B, Arnal LH, Schroeder CE, Keitel A. Prominence of delta oscillatory rhythms in the motor cortex and their relevance for auditory and speech perception. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2019;107:136–42.
Murrar S, Brauer M. Mixed-model analysis of variance. In: Frey BB, editor. The SAGE encyclopedia of educational research, measurement, and evaluation. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE; 2018. p. 1075–8.
Neef NE, Anwander A, Bütfering C, Schmidt-Samoa C, Friederici AD, Paulus W, Sommer M. Structural connectivity of right frontal hyperactive areas scales with stuttering severity. Brain. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awx316.
Neef NE, Anwander A, Friederici AD. The neurobiological grounding of persistent stuttering: from structure to function. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-015-0579-4.
Neef NE, Jung K, Rothkegel H, Pollok B, von Gudenberg AW, Paulus W, Sommer M. Right-shift for non-speech motor processing in adults who stutter. Cortex. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.007.
Ohara S, Mima T, Baba K, Ikeda A, Kunieda T, Matsumoto R, et al. Increased synchronization of cortical oscillatory activities between human supplementary motor and primary sensorimotor areas during voluntary movements. J Neurosci. 2001;21:9377–86.
Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.
Özge A, Toros F, Çömelekoglu Ü. The role of hemispheral asymmetry and regional activity of quantitative EEG in children with stuttering. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:chud.0000020679.15106.a4.
Pfurtscheller G, Lopes da Silva FH. Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin Neurophysiol. 1999;Â Â https://doi.org/10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00141-8. PMID: 10576479.
Pfurtscheller G, Stancák AJr, Neuper C. Post-movement beta synchronization. A correlate of an idling motor area? Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1996;  https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(95)00258-8
Piispala J, Kallio M, Bloigu R, Jansson-Verkasalo E. Delayed N2 response in Go condition in a visual Go/Nogo ERP study in children who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2016;48:16–26.
Pollak SD, Wolfe BL. Maximizing research on the adverse effects of child poverty through consensus measures. Dev Sci. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12946.
Popovych S, Rosjat N, Toth TI, Wang BA, Liu L, Abdollahi RO, Viswanathan S, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Daun S. Movement-related phase locking in the delta-theta frequency band. Neuroimage. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.052.
Preibisch C, Neumann K, Raab P, Euler HA, von Gudenberg AW, Lanfermann H, Giraud AL. Evidence for compensation for stuttering by the right frontal operculum. Neuroimage. 2003;20(2):1356–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00376-8. (PMID: 14568504).
Ramayya AG, Yang AI, Buch VP, Burke JF, Richardson AG, Brandon C, Stein JM, Davis KA, Chen HI, Proekt A, Kelz MB, Litt B, Gold JI, Lucas TH. Theta synchrony is increased near neural populations that are active when initiating instructed movement. eNeuro. 2021;Â Â https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0252-20.2020
Riley G. The Stuttering Severity Instrument for Adults and Children (SSI-4). 4th ed. Austin: PRO-ED; 2009.
Rastatter MP, Stuart A, Kalinowski J. Quantitative electroencephalogram of posterior cortical areas of fluent and stuttering participants during reading with normal and altered auditory feedback. Percept Mot Skills. 1998;87(2):623–33.
Rosjat N, Liu L, Wang BA, Popovych S, Tóth T, Viswanathan S, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Daun S. Aging-associated changes of movement-related functional connectivity in the human brain. Neuropsychologia. 2018;117:520–9.
Rosjat N, Wang BA, Liu L, Fink GR, Daun S. Stimulus transformation into motor action: Dynamic graph analysis reveals a posterior-to-anterior shift in brain network communication of older subjects. Hum Brain Mapp. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25313.
Salmelin R, Hari R. Spatiotemporal characteristics of sensorimotor neuromagnetic rhythms related to thumb movement. Neuroscience. 1994. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(94)90263-1.
Salmelin R, Schnitzler A, Schmitz F, Freund HJ. Single word reading in developmental stutterers and fluent speakers. Brain. 2000. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.6.1184.
Saltuklaroglu T, Bowers A, Harkrider AW, Casenhiser D, Reilly KJ, Jenson DE, Thornton D. EEG mu rhythms: rich sources of sensorimotor information in speech processing. Brain Lang. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2018.09.005.
Sauseng P, Klimesch W. What does phase information of oscillatory brain activity tell us about cognitive processes? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.03.014.
Schmidt R, Herrojo Ruiz M, Kilavik BE, Lundqvist M, Starr PA, Aron AR. Beta oscillations in working memory, executive control of movement and thought, and sensorimotor function. J Neuroscience. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1163-19.2019.
Schnitzler A, Gross J. Normal and pathological oscillatory communication in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1650.
Seeber M, Scherer R, Müller-Putz GR. EEG oscillations are modulated in different behavior-related networks during rhythmic finger movements. J Neurosci. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1739-16.2016.
Sengupta R, Nasir SM. Redistribution of neural phase coherence reflects establishment of feedforward map in speech motor adaptation. J Neurophysiol. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00731.2014.
Sengupta R, Shah S, Loucks TMJ, Pelczarski K, Scott Yaruss J, Gore K, Nasir SM. Cortical dynamics of disfluency in adults who stutter. Physiol Rep. 2017. https://doi.org/10.14814/phy2.13194.
Sengupta R, Yaruss JS, Loucks TM, Gracco VL, Pelczarski K, Nasir SM. Theta modulated neural phase coherence facilitates speech fluency in adults who stutter. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00394.
Serrien DJ, Brown P. The functional role of interhemispheric synchronization in the control of bimanual timing tasks. Exp Brain Res. 2002;147:268–72.
Smith A, Goffman L, Sasisekaran J, Weber-Fox C. Language and motor abilities of preschool children who stutter: evidence from behavioral and kinematic indices of nonword repetition performance. Journal of fluency disorders. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.06.001.
Smith A, Weber C. How stuttering develops: the multifactorial dynamic pathways theory. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-16-0343.
Smits-Bandstra S, De Nil L, Rochon E. The transition to increased automaticity during finger sequence learning in adult males who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2005.11.004.
Smits-Bandstra S, De Nil LF, Saint-Cyr JA. Speech and nonspeech sequence skill learning in adults who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.04.003.
Sowman PF, Crain S, Harrison E, Johnson BW. Reduced activation of left orbitofrontal cortex precedes blocked vocalization: a magnetoencephalographic study. J Fluency Disord. 2012;37(4):359–65.
Sowman PF, Crain S, Harrison E, Johnson BW. Lateralization of brain activation in fluent and non-fluent preschool children: a magnetoencephalographic study of picture-naming. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;28(8):354. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00354.PMID:24904388;PMCID:PMC4035571.
Tan H, Wade C, Brown P. Post-movement beta activity in sensorimotor cortex indexes confidence in the estimations from internal models. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3204-15.2016.
Toyomura A, Fujii T, Sowman PF. Performance of bimanual finger coordination tasks in speakers who stutter. Front Psychol. 2021. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.679607.
Vanhoutte S, Santens P, Cosyns M, van Mierlo P, Batens K, Corthals P, De Letter M, Van Borsel J. Increased motor preparation activity during fluent single word production in DS: a correlate for stuttering frequency and severity. Neuropsychologia. 2015;75:1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.013. (Epub 2015 May 21 PMID: 26004061).
Vanhoutte S, Cosyns M, van Mierlo P, Batens K, Corthals P, De Letter M, Van Borsel J, Santens P. When will a stuttering moment occur? The determining role of speech motor preparation. Neuropsychologia. 2016;86:93–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.04.018. (Epub 2016 Apr 19 PMID: 27106391).
van Wijk BC, Beek PJ, Daffertshofer A. Neural synchrony within the motor system: what have we learned so far? Front Hum Neurosci. 2012. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00252.
Varela F, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001. https://doi.org/10.1038/35067550.
Vinck M, Oostenveld R, van Wingerden M, Battaglia F, Pennartz CMA. An improved index of phase-synchronization for electrophysiological data in the presence of volume-conduction, noise and sample-size bias. NeuroImage. 2011;55(4):1548–65.
von Nicolai C, Engler G, Sharott A, Engel AK. Moll CK and Siegel M. Corticostriatal coordination through coherent phase-amplitude coupling. J Neuroscience. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5007-13.2014.
Wiig EH, Secord WA, Semel EM. Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals preschool-2. Pearson. 2004
Wiig EH, Semel EM, Secord WA. Clinicalevaluation of language fundamentals: Fifth edition. Pearson. 2013
Wiltshire CEE, Chiew M, Chesters J, Healy MP, Watkins KE. Speech movement variability in people who stutter: a vocal tract magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2021;Â Â https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_JSLHR-20-00507
Yairi E, Seery C. Stuttering: foundations and clinical applications. Pearson Education, Inc. 2011
Yeom HG, Kim JS, Chung CK. Brain mechanisms in motor control during reaching movements: Transition of functional connectivity according to movement states. Sci Rep. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-57489-7.
Zeng K, Drummond NM, Ghahremani A, Saha U, Kalia SK, Hodaie M, Lozano AM, Aron AR, Chen R. Fronto-subthalamic phase synchronization and cross-frequency coupling during conflict processing. NeuroImage. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118205.
Acknowledgements
We thank all the children who have participated in this study. We thank Megan Sheppard and Saralyn Rubsam for their help with participant recruitment and behavioral assessment administration.
Funding
This work was supported by grants from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD R01DC018283 awarded to SE. Chang and NIDCD R21DC015875 awarded to A. Hampton Wray).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
VCC analyzed and interpreted the data and wrote the manuscript. EL collected the data. VCC, SEC, AHW, and EL made substantial contributions to the design of the work, to the interpretation of the data, and to writing/editing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All study procedures were approved by the Human Research Protection Program at Michigan State University. Prior to data collection, all children’s primary caregivers gave written informed consent, and children gave assent to participate in the study.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Caruso, V.C., Wray, A.H., Lescht, E. et al. Neural oscillatory activity and connectivity in children who stutter during a non-speech motor task. J Neurodevelop Disord 15, 40 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-023-09507-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-023-09507-8