- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Language delay aggregates in toddler siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder
Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders volume 10, Article number: 29 (2018)
Abstract
Background
Language delay is extremely common in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), yet it is unclear whether measurable variation in early language is associated with genetic liability for ASD. Assessment of language development in unaffected siblings of children with ASD can inform whether decreased early language ability aggregates with inherited risk for ASD and serves as an ASD endophenotype.
Methods
We implemented two approaches: (1) a meta-analysis of studies comparing language delay, a categorical indicator of language function, and language scores, a continuous metric, in unaffected toddlers at high and low familial risk for ASD, and (2) a parallel analysis of 350 unaffected 24-month-olds in the Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS), a prospective study of infants at high and low familial risk for ASD. An advantage of the former was its detection of group differences from pooled data across unique samples; an advantage of the latter was its sensitivity in quantifying early manifestations of language delay while accounting for covariates within a single large sample.
Results
Meta-analysis showed that high-risk siblings without ASD (HR-noASD) were three to four times more likely to exhibit language delay versus low-risk siblings without ASD (LR-noASD) and had lower mean receptive and expressive language scores. Analyses of IBIS data corroborated that language delay, specifically receptive language delay, was more frequent in the HR-noASD (n = 235) versus LR-noASD group (n = 115). IBIS language scores were continuously and unimodally distributed, with a pathological shift towards decreased language function in HR-noASD siblings. The elevated inherited risk for ASD was associated with lower receptive and expressive language scores when controlling for sociodemographic factors. For receptive but not expressive language, the effect of risk group remained significant even when controlling for nonverbal cognition.
Conclusions
Greater frequency of language delay and a lower distribution of language scores in high-risk, unaffected toddler-aged siblings support decreased early language ability as an endophenotype for ASD, with a more pronounced effect for receptive versus expressive language. Further characterization of language development is warranted to refine genetic investigations of ASD and to elucidate factors influencing the progression of core autistic traits and related symptoms.
Background
A major challenge in elucidating the biology underlying autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is its genetic heterogeneity. Endophenotypes, heritable characteristics which share genetic liability with a disorder and which are measurable regardless of a disorder’s state or stage (i.e., state-independent) [1, 2], are therefore especially informative for resolving the complex, polygenic genetic architecture of ASD. By definition, endophenotypes demonstrate several criteria involving inheritance among family members with and without the disorder. These criteria include co-segregating, or being inherited more commonly, in affected versus unaffected family members and aggregating, or occurring with increased frequency, in unaffected family members versus the general population, which is at lower genetic risk [1, 2]. The occurrence of these familial patterns in the context of a heritable trait substantiates the relationship between an endophenotype and genetic factors for a given disorder. Because endophenotypes can be inferred to reflect causal pathways of a disorder and can be reliably measured in individuals with and without the disorder [1, 2], they enhance the sensitivity to determine contributory genes and, by extension, the underlying biology.
The common co-occurrence of ASD and persistent language impairments [3], which may include deficits in aspects of structural language, such as vocabulary and grammar, as well as pragmatics, the appropriate use of language, has prompted the long-standing question of whether language deficits represent an endophenotype of ASD [4, 5]. Like ASD, language disorders are heritable [6,7,8,9], with evidence of genetic influence from early in development [10, 11]. Further, as expected for an endophenotype, both autistic symptoms and language ability appear to behave as quantitative traits which are heritable across a range of competency encompassing unimpaired and impaired individuals [9, 12,13,14,15]. Multiple family studies have investigated the potential role of language function as an ASD endophenotype by examining whether language impairment occurs with increased frequency in families with a history of ASD. Although several of these studies have reported that unaffected family members of individuals with ASD show more language impairment than expected for the general population [16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25], others have failed to find a difference from expected rates in a control population [26,27,28,29]. Conversely, other groups have found an increased prevalence of ASD in family members of individuals with a specific language impairment (SLI) [30, 31], but again, this finding has not been universal [27, 32]. Interpretation of the literature is complicated by several factors limiting the comparability across studies, including differing diagnostic criteria for language impairment (e.g., [26] versus [22]); small sample sizes [23, 29]; broad participant age ranges [33], which may mask developmentally sensitive manifestations of language function; and lack of a control group [24] or standardized language assessment [21, 25]. Given this inconsistency, the field has been challenged to arrive at a consensus regarding whether language function operates as an ASD endophenotype.
Related work examining quantitative relationships between autistic traits and language function generally supports overlapping genetic factors, in keeping with a language-related ASD endophenotype. For example, a recent general population twin study found shared genetic influences for early childhood language scores at age 2 years and quantitative measures of autistic traits at school age [34]. In a study measuring autistic traits in unaffected siblings of individuals with ASD [35], higher levels of autistic traits were observed in siblings with a history of language delay, again supporting overlapping genetic influences for ASD and language delay, as well as an amplification of ASD risk with a co-occurring history of language delay. Multiple genetic studies have identified genes, such as contactin-associated protein-like 2 (CNTNAP2) [36,37,38], and genetic loci, particularly on chromosome 7 [39,40,41,42,43], which are associated with both ASD and specific language impairment. These convergent findings imply that shared genes may lead to disruptions of social and language development, and language delay is frequently observed in ASD [44,45,46]. A relevant question is therefore whether language delay, which entails altered emergence of foundational language skills (e.g., comprehension and production of words, word combinations, and simple sentences), and which displays some continuity with later language function [10, 47,48,49,50], aggregates in unaffected toddler siblings of individuals with ASD. Identification and characterization of an early language endophenotype has implications for enhancing diagnostic sensitivity and risk stratification, clarifying developmental mechanisms, and refining targets for early interventions.
To test whether early language delay is an endophenotype of ASD, we leveraged data from infant sibling studies, developmental family studies designed to identify early predictive risk factors of ASD. In these genetically informative study designs, infant siblings of children with ASD, who are at elevated familial risk for ASD, as well as sibling controls at low familial risk for ASD, undergo standardized behavioral and diagnostic testing. Because these studies generally involve similar ages and assessments, comparable data can be pooled across samples to improve power to detect traits associated with inherited ASD risk. By comparing risk groups, endophenotypes can be identified as features enriched in those at elevated familial risk of ASD. Evaluating differences in language function between high-risk siblings without ASD (HR-noASD siblings) and low-risk siblings without ASD (LR-noASD siblings) affords a particularly stringent test for an endophenotype, since differences between these unaffected groups are not biased by the high comorbidity of language deficits in ASD [44,45,46] and are attributable to the presence of familial ASD liability. Additionally, this comparison disambiguates the role of inherited ASD risk, which is linked to underlying mechanisms of ASD, from consequences of ASD itself, thereby facilitating investigation of the role of language-related factors, such as sociodemographic variables, nonverbal cognition, and ASD-related social deficits, in the manifestation of an endophenotype.
Given extant literature on HR-noASD infant siblings, we conducted a meta-analysis as a first step to test whether language delay, as well as lower mean language scores, aggregate in HR-noASD toddlers. We focused on the toddler period since broad variation in advancing language abilities at this stage could enhance detection of group-level differences. We hypothesized that if disruptions in early language associate with inherited ASD risk, a greater frequency of language delay and lower mean language scores would be observed in HR-noASD siblings versus LR-noASD siblings. Based on the results of this meta-analysis, we tested whether observed differences could be replicated and extended using data from the Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS), a large infant sibling study [51]. The IBIS cohort expanded the sample for meta-analysis while allowing more comprehensive analyses which controlled for sociodemographic factors not universally reported in published studies, examined the distribution of language scores for each risk group, and investigated the relationship between language and nonverbal cognition, as well as language and ASD-related social deficits. We hypothesized that if language delay were an endophenotype, the HR-noASD group in IBIS would show an increased prevalence of language delay versus the LR-noASD group, a downward shift in distributions of language scores, and lower mean language scores versus the LR-noASD group, even when sociodemographic factors were controlled.
Methods
Literature review
To review the available literature for evidence of associations between familial ASD risk and decreased language, we searched for published articles both in PubMed, a database with strong representation of clinical literature, and Scopus, a database with broader coverage in the social sciences [52]. The search used the keywords “autism,” “language,” and “sibling” for manuscripts published since 2000, the year the DSM-IV-TR was published [53]. Inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) analysis of a high-risk group of toddlers, here considered children with a mean age of 12–24 months, who had a sibling with ASD but no ASD diagnosis themselves (HR-noASD siblings), (2) analysis of a low-risk group of toddlers who had a typically developing sibling and no ASD diagnosis themselves (LR-noASD siblings), (3) a clinical best estimate diagnosis of ASD for children aged 24 months and up, an early age with evidence for diagnostic stability [54,55,56,57], (4) implementation of standardized language assessments from 1 to 2 years of age, as this particular period captures variation in the early emergence of spoken language, and (5) reporting of language scores or standardized criteria for language delay for both HR-noASD and LR-noASD groups. Studies without a diagnostic evaluation of toddlers below age 24 months, when symptoms of ASD are less likely to have stably emerged [58, 59], were retained for consideration in the meta-analysis to promote broader representation of unique study populations, which enhances the generalizability of findings in a meta-analysis [60].
The search identified 216 articles published between January 1, 2000, and May 31, 2017. One hundred nineteen of these involved HR-noASD siblings. Fifty-seven articles reported on toddlers (i.e., children with a mean age of 12–24 months), and 52 of these examined aspects of language development. Twenty-six of the 52 articles satisfied inclusion criteria. Among these 26 articles, 15 articles were eliminated because they shared participants with another study, either due to multiple manuscripts about the same population or due to manuscripts describing a consortium of studies. The remaining 11 articles represented data from all samples in the eliminated articles and were chosen due to having the largest number of children closest to 24 months of age, a common assessment time point expected to exhibit a broader range of spoken language abilities than younger ages. Where language data were available from multiple measures within a study, continuous scores from the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) [61], the most frequent assessment encountered, were selected to enhance comparability across studies as well as IBIS, which featured MSEL data. One study [62], which reported age equivalent scores on the MSEL but did not control for age, was excluded to avoid confounding age differences with differences in language ability. To promote uniformity in the meta-analysis of continuous language scores, one additional study population was excluded [63, 64], as it used the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories [65], a parent-report measure.
A large study from the Baby Sibling Research Consortium (BSRC) [66], which was included in the main meta-analysis presented in Fig. 1, differed from other studies in that it reported continuous language scores as estimated marginal means (rather than T-scores) based on a model testing effects of sex, age, language subscale, and diagnostic group on language outcome. This study shared subjects with some smaller studies containing appropriate continuous language data that were excluded from the main meta-analysis of continuous scores [45, 67, 68]. For purposes of comparison, supplemental analyses repeat the meta-analysis using these smaller studies instead of this large BSRC study and show consistent results (Additional file 1: Table S1 and Figure S1). Note that one of these smaller studies, Mitchell et al. [45], contained categorical data on language delay, which were included in that segment of the meta-analysis reported in the main text (Table 1).
Lastly, meta-analytic findings from the literature review are first presented without incorporating IBIS data, since these results motivated subsequent analyses in IBIS to test for replicability and the impact of covariates on group differences.
IBIS sample
The Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS) is a longitudinal multisite study of infants at familial risk of ASD by virtue of having a sibling with a diagnosis of ASD, verified by medical records and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [69]. IBIS also contains a low-risk comparison group of infant siblings, who have no first-degree family members meeting screening criteria for ASD or intellectual disability [51]. Rates of ASD diagnosis in IBIS [51, 70] have been shown to be similar to other infant sibling studies [71]. Participants in the first wave of IBIS, whose data were used in these analyses, were behaviorally assessed and completed magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) during natural sleep at ages 6, 12, and 24 months at the following study sites: the University of North Carolina, the University of Washington, The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, and Washington University in St. Louis. The Montreal Neurological Institute served as the data coordination center. Exclusion criteria included: (1) diagnosis or physical signs of known genetic conditions or syndromes, (2) significant medical or neurological conditions or sensory impairments, (3) birth weight < 2000 g and/or gestational age < 36 weeks, (4) significant perinatal adversity and/or exposure in utero to neurotoxins, (5) contraindication for MRI, (6) predominant home language other than English, (7) first degree relative with psychosis, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and (8) adopted children or half-siblings of the proband. The majority of analyses presented here involve HR-noASD siblings and LR-noASD siblings. High-risk siblings with ASD were included in a single sub-analysis testing differences between HR-noASD siblings and high-risk siblings with ASD. Informed consent approved by each site’s Human Subjects Review Board was obtained for all families.
Measures
Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL)
The MSEL is a standardized direct assessment of cognitive development normed for ages from birth to 68 months [61]. Subscales include receptive and expressive language, as well as visual receptive and motor skills. Mullen T-scores, based on standardized norms accounting for age, were used to index levels of receptive and expressive language function. Language delay was defined as a T-score ≤ 35, or 1.5 standard deviations below the mean standard score, in accordance with common practice [70, 72], on either receptive or expressive language subscales. A nonverbal composite score was created by averaging the T-scores for visual reception and fine motor subscales.
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS)
The ADOS [73] is a semi-structured play assessment of characteristic features of ASD in the domains of communication, social interaction, play skills, and restricted interests/repetitive behavior. ADOS module 1 or 2, designed for different levels of language development, was administered to all subjects at 24 months by certified evaluators who were research reliable across all four sites [51]. Ratings were based on the severity and number of ASD symptoms demonstrated during the assessment, and scores were calculated using empirically derived, conventional scoring algorithms comprised of items identified as strong contributors to variance in prior factor analysis of the ADOS [74]. To maximize the range of detectable variation in children without ASD, the summed item-level scores from the ADOS social affect scoring algorithm, based on a previously identified ADOS social affect factor measuring ASD-related social deficits [74], were used as an index of social performance. Higher social affect scores corresponded to an increased burden of ASD-related deficits. In the supplement, results for analyses using calibrated severity scores for social affect [75] are presented with similar findings (Additional file 1: Table S2).
DSM-IV-TR checklist
Diagnoses of ASD were made using a clinical best estimate diagnosis derived from the IBIS behavioral battery and observations during in-person assessment, including the ADOS [51]. Testing, video, and interview data were reviewed by a second experienced clinician to confirm that criteria for an ASD [(autism or Pervasive Developmental Disorder not otherwise specified (NOS)] were met using the DSM-IV-TR checklist at 24 months [53].
Statistical analyses
The Mantel-Haenszel test, a meta-analytic technique for categorical data [76], was used to evaluate proportions of language delay in HR-noASD versus LR-noASD siblings. To compare language scores in these risk groups across studies, a meta-analysis was performed using a random effects model with inverse variance weighting [77]. First, effect sizes of the differences in language scores between LR-noASD and HR-noASD groups were calculated for each study. Study-specific inverse variance weights, which accounted for sample size and standard error, were then derived, with larger sample size and lower standard errors corresponding to greater inverse variance weight. To standardize the impact of each study in the meta-analysis, each study’s effect size was multiplied by its inverse variance weight. These products were summed and divided by the sum of inverse variant weights for all studies to determine a summary effect size or standardized mean difference. A random effects model was conservatively chosen to account for variance between and within studies, although cross-study heterogeneity was not significant based on the Cochran Q statistic [77] (receptive language Q = 0.75, df = 4, p = .95; expressive language Q = 3.48, df = 4, p = .48). Age and sociodemographic variables (the latter of which were not uniformly available across studies) were not tested as covariates in these models due to the modest number of studies (< 10), which constrains the ability to accurately estimate the impact of potential moderators through meta-regression [60, 78].
Within the IBIS dataset, differences in participant characteristics between HR-noASD and LR-noASD groups at age 24 months (and in one sub-analysis, between HR-noASD siblings and high-risk siblings with ASD) were examined using t tests or χ2 tests where appropriate for continuous or categorical variables. Children with significant generalized cognitive delay, indicated by a nonverbal developmental composite score ≥ 2 standard deviations below the mean (a level in the bottom 5% of the population) were removed from analyses (HR-noASD n = 1; LR-noASD n = 1). Binary logistic regression, with ASD-risk status (HR-noASD versus LR-noASD) as the independent variable, was used to test for differences in the presence of language delay (categorized as having or not having language delay), the dependent variable, while controlling for the sociodemographic factors of sex, maternal education (categorized according to those with and without a college degree), income (categorized as greater or less than $75,000 per year), and race (categorized as Caucasian or not Caucasian), as these variables have generally been found to be associated with early language development [79,80,81,82,83]. Sociodemographic factors were entered prior to ASD risk status in these models. Hierarchical linear regressions, with language scores as the dependent variable, tested the influence of ASD risk status, the independent variable, on language ability when controlling for sociodemographic factors, as described above. Additional hierarchical linear regressions examined contributions of nonverbal cognition (nonverbal composite score) and social performance (ADOS social affect score) to variation in language scores. Correlation values for language scores with other behavioral scores were Fisher z-transformed to test for significant differences between HR-noASD and LR-noASD groups.
Results
Meta-analysis: review of the literature for associations between ASD risk and early language
Our literature review identified nine infant sibling studies with standardized language data in HR-noASD and LR-noASD siblings (Table 1). Four of these studies contained categorical data on the presence of language delay (see Table 1 for individual study criteria), for a total of 181 HR-noASD siblings and 133 LR-noASD siblings. Although all four studies displayed odds ratios consistent with greater language delay in HR-noASD siblings, many of the samples were small, and only two studies showed statistically significant differences in odds ratios between the two risk groups. The Mantel-Haenszel test, which allowed pooling of participants across these studies, showed that HR-noASD siblings were 4.17 (95% CI 1.74–9.99) times more likely to experience language delay than LR-noASD siblings [χ2MH(1) = 14.62, p < .001; LR-noASD 6.0% language delay; HR-noASD 21.0% language delay].
For five of the studies (Table 1), comparison of early language ability between HR-noASD and LR-noASD siblings was possible based on continuous scores from the MSEL. These scores provided enhanced sensitivity relative to categorical data for examining risk group differences in receptive and expressive language. Across studies, mean language scores for both groups (Table 1) fell within a normative range (within 1 SD, 10 points, of a mean standard MSEL T-score of 50), although scores were generally lower for the high-risk group. A meta-analysis (Fig. 1) using weighted effect sizes for studies including 1164 HR-noASD siblings and 707 LR-noASD siblings indicated significantly higher receptive language for LR-noASD siblings, with a standardized mean difference (i.e., summary effect size) of 0.48 (95% confidence interval 0.39–0.58). For expressive language, a standardized mean difference of 0.30 (95% confidence interval 0.21–0.40) also indicated significantly higher scores for LR-noASD siblings. Similar effect sizes were observed in a secondary meta-analysis substituting the larger BSRC study with smaller studies sharing some of the same subjects (see Additional file 1: Figure S1 and Table S1). Meta-analysis of the available literature thus suggests that around the age of identification of core ASD symptoms, HR-noASD siblings exhibit more frequent language delay and lower receptive and expressive language scores than low-risk counterparts.
The Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS): testing for replication of language differences in HR-noASD siblings
While the meta-analysis provides evidence for the aggregation of decreased early language skills in unaffected high-risk siblings, the diversity of participant ages and assessments could inflate variability in language measurements, potentially leading to underestimation of differences between risk groups. Additionally, studies with participants under age 24 months, below the usual age of assessment for ASD, included children who could later be diagnosed with ASD. Therefore, we investigated whether a similar result would be observed in the Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS), a large infant sibling study sample. IBIS data allowed the analysis of covariates not uniformly available from studies in the meta-analysis as well as augmentation of the meta-analysis. We analyzed children without ASD at 24 months of age, an age anticipated to capture a wider range of measurable variation in language than younger ages (e.g., 12 months, when children are in the process of learning single words). Sample characteristics (n = 350) are shown in Table 2. HR-noASD (n = 235) and LR-noASD (n = 115) did not significantly differ in age, sex, income, or race and showed similar means and standard deviations for the ADOS social affect score. Significant differences between risk groups were observed in maternal education, a nonverbal developmental composite score, and receptive and expressive language scores. The high-risk group showed lower MSEL scores, a higher prevalence of language delay, and a lower percentage of mothers with college or graduate degrees. Comparison of IBIS HR-noASD siblings to high-risk siblings with ASD revealed lower language function in the ASD-affected group, confirming that the IBIS sample is appropriately representative to test a candidate endophenotype (Additional file 1: Supplemental Results).
Comparison of language delay in HR-noASD and LR-noASD siblings in IBIS
To account for sociodemographic factors associated with language outcomes, including sex, maternal education, and income, we performed a binary logistic regression comparing the prevalence of language delay in the LR-noASD and HR-noASD siblings in IBIS. Log of the odds of language delay (coded for as the presence or absence of receptive or expressive language delay) served as the dependent variable. ASD risk group was entered after controlling for sociodemographic variables. The model was significant (χ2(5) = 13.35, p = .02) and showed appropriate goodness of fit (Hosmer’s and Lemeshow’s test χ2(8) = 3.32, p = .91). ASD risk status was a significant contributor to the model (χ2Wald(1) = 5.21, p = .022) and accounted for 3.7% of the variance in language delay status, with HR-noASD being 3.18 times (95% CI = 1.18–8.59) more likely than LR-noASD to have a language delay. Sex, maternal education, income, and race were not significant contributors.
To test whether receptive and/or expressive language individually contributed to this effect, analyses were repeated separately for receptive and expressive language delay. The model for receptive language delay was also significant (χ2(5) = 20.32, p = .001) and showed good fit (Hosmer’s and Lemeshow’s test χ2(7) = 3.24, p = .86). Risk status accounted for 5.7% of the variance in receptive language, and HR-noASD siblings were 5.82 times (95% CI 1.30–26.05) more likely to have receptive language delay (χ2Wald(1) = 5.30, p = .021). The model for expressive language delay was not significant (χ2(5) = 6.71, p = .24).
Integration of IBIS data in the meta-analysis of language delay
Inclusion of IBIS percentages for language delay (receptive and/or expressive) in the meta-analysis increased the precision of the findings, with HR-noASD siblings (n = 416) being 3.87 times (95% CI 2.04–7.33) more likely to have a language delay than LR-noASD siblings (n = 248; χ2MH(1) = 21.32, p < .001; HR-noASD 17.0% language delay; LR-noASD 5.2% language delay). When excluding the study of children under age 24 months without diagnostic data [83], the odds ratio was similar [3.29 (95% CI 1.40–7.69), χ2MH(1) = 14.58, p < .001; HR-noASD n = 402, 15.7% language delay; LR-noASD n = 230, 5.7% language delay]. Integration of IBIS with the literature confirms that language delay does aggregate in unaffected, toddler-aged, high-risk siblings.
Comparison of language scores in HR-noASD and LR-noASD siblings in IBIS
Findings from the meta-analysis do not distinguish whether observed differences in ASD risk groups are attributable to a subgroup of HR-noASD siblings with language impairment or a shift of continuous score distributions towards decreased language function across all HR-noASD siblings. Such a pathological shift would be expected for an endophenotype encompassing the full range of language function, with language delay representing a pathological extreme. Therefore, we next examined the language score distributions for high- and low-risk siblings in IBIS without ASD. Histograms of T-scores on the MSEL receptive and expressive language subscales demonstrated a continuous, unimodal distribution for both groups (Fig. 2). Visual inspection of binned scores showed that HR-noASD siblings had a smaller proportion of high scores than LR-noASD siblings and a larger proportion of low scores, corresponding to a pathological shift in score distributions for the HR-ASD siblings. Comparison of proportions for the HR-noASD versus LR-noASD siblings in the top and bottom thirds of the sample was consistent with this downward shift in scores (HR-noASD versus LR-noASD in the top third—receptive 26.4% versus 47.8%, χ2(1) = 5.71, p = 0.017; expressive 26.0% versus 48.7%, χ2(1) = 6.41, p = 0.011; in the bottom third—receptive 39.6% versus 20.9%, χ2(1) = 2.88, p = 0.090; expressive 42.1% versus 15.7%, χ2(1) = 4.47, p = 0.035). Additionally, HR-noASD siblings demonstrated lower mean receptive and expressive language scores than LR-noASD siblings (Table 2) at effect sizes similar to those observed in the meta-analysis (receptive Cohen’s d = .54 and expressive Cohen’s d = .38).
We next implemented a hierarchical linear regression to test whether language scores, the dependent variable, were predicted by ASD risk status, the independent variable. These models controlled for sociodemographic variables, which were entered first. Receptive and expressive language were analyzed separately, given the distinct effect sizes for these two language domains in the meta-analysis. In the case of receptive language (Table 3), ASD risk status, sex, and income were significant contributors to the model (F(5,331) = 10.45, p < .001), which accounted for 12.5% of the variance (adjusted R2) in receptive language scores, with risk status contributing 4.4% of the variance.
As with receptive language, the effect of ASD risk status was significant for expressive language when controlling for sociodemographic variables (Table 3). The total model explained a similar amount of the variance in expressive language, 8.3%, [F(5,331) = 6.96, p < .001], with risk status accounting for approximately 1.6% of that variance. Thus, in keeping with results for language delay, familial risk of ASD was associated with lower language scores even when controlling for variation attributable to these sociodemographic factors.
Integration of IBIS data in the meta-analysis of quantitative language scores
Inclusion of the IBIS sample in the meta-analysis of MSEL language scores increased the precision of summary effect sizes for both receptive and expressive language [receptive 0.49 (95% CI 0.40–0.58); expressive 0.31 (95% CI 0.23–0.40)], based on a total of 1399 HR-noASD and 822 LR-noASD siblings (Fig. 1). Removing the studies of toddlers under 24 months without diagnostic data (86–88) yielded similar values [receptive language 0.49 (95% confidence interval 0.40–0.58); expressive language 0.30 (95% confidence interval 0.21–0.40); HR-noASD n = 1266; LR-noASD n = 718].
Examination of the specificity of a language endophenotype in IBIS
The above findings demonstrate an association of familial ASD risk with decreased early language function, consistent with a language endophenotype of ASD. To investigate whether ASD risk status was related to variation specific to language, or aspects of language associated with nonverbal cognitive ability, a known predictor of language [84,85,86], we next evaluated the effect of ASD risk status when nonverbal cognitive ability was included in the model. The MSEL nonverbal composite score was introduced as a third step in the models. In the case of receptive language, the model was significant (F(6,331) = 27.10, p < .001) and accounted for 32.1% of the variance (adjusted R2), with nonverbal cognition adding 19.6% variance. Even with the inclusion of the nonverbal score, the effect of ASD risk status on receptive language remained significant (beta = − 2.09; SE = 1.03 p = .044). In contrast to receptive language, the effect of risk status for expressive language was no longer significant (beta = −.44; SE = 1.16 p = .71), with the nonverbal composite accounting for 18.6% of the variance in expressive language (F(6,331) = 21.27, p < .001; beta = .59; SE = .064 p < .001).
Examination of the relationship between ASD-related social features and language in IBIS
An important issue for genetic discovery in ASD is the extent to which distinct genetic factors account for variation in ASD-related phenotypes. While the findings above support overlapping genetic factors for ASD and language delay, they do not address the extent of this overlap. In the case of high overlap, most of the same genetic factors would account for variation in language and social behavior, whereas in the case of lower partial overlap, some genes which contribute to ASD risk would exert independent effects on social and language phenotypes. We therefore tested whether the ADOS social affect score, an index of social performance quantifying ASD-related social deficits, influenced relationships between ASD risk and language scores. We reasoned that if the effect of ASD risk group on language was no longer significant when accounting for social affect score, then core ASD-related social symptoms and language function share a high degree of overlapping genetic factors. Alternatively, should the effect of ASD risk group on language persist when accounting for social affect score, then shared genetic contributors to autistic social features and language function would appear partially independent at this age.
A final step of hierarchical regression models is shown in Table 4. As above, models controlled for sex, maternal education, income, and race in the first step, followed by a second step adding risk group and a third step adding social affect score. Models including social affect score were significant for both receptive and expressive language (receptive F(7,327) = 9.65, p < .001; expressive F(7,327) = 7.27, p < .001), and social affect score contributed to variation in both receptive and expressive language. The contribution of ASD risk group to both receptive and expressive language remained significant with social affect score included (Table 4), consistent with partially independent genetic factors accounting for early ASD-related deficits in language and social behavior (see Additional file 1: Supplemental Results and Table S1, for consistent findings using the ADOS calibrated severity social affect scores).
The final step tested an interaction between ASD risk group and social affect score, as similar mean social affect scores for both LR-noASD and HR-noASD siblings (Table 1; see Additional file 1: Figure S2 for score distributions) implied a preservation of social performance relative to language in HR-noASD siblings. We therefore tested an interaction to determine whether the nature of a relationship between language and ASD-related social deficits differed between the risk groups. A significant interaction was found for expressive language (p = .002), as well as a trend-level interaction effect for receptive language (p = .056) (Table 4). Given these results, we next examined correlations for social affect and language scores separately for LR-noASD and HR-noASD groups. LR-noASD siblings showed moderate correlations between language and social affect scores (receptive: Spearman’s rho = − .33, p < .001.; expressive Spearman’s rho = − .41, p < .001), such that higher social affect scores, corresponding to greater ASD-related social deficits, were associated with lower language scores. In contrast, HR-noASD siblings exhibited no significant correlations between language and social affect scores (receptive: Spearman’s rho = − .069, p = .29; expressive: Spearman’s rho = − .051, p = .44; see Additional file 1: Supplemental Results, for similar findings when analyzing high-risk males and females separately). These differences in correlations for language and social affect scores were significant between LR-noASD and HR-noASD groups (receptive language: Z = − 2.33, p = .02; expressive language: Z = − 3.34, p < .001). By comparison, moderate correlations were present in both groups for language scores and nonverbal cognition, a developmental domain also related to language (LR-noASD receptive: Spearman’s rho = .62, p < .001; LR-noASD expressive: Spearman’s rho = .42, p < .001; HR-noASD receptive Spearman’s rho = .49, p < .001; HR-noASD expressive: Spearman’s rho = .53, p < .001).
Discussion
Convergent approaches support language endophenotypes of ASD
These findings demonstrate that decreased language function in early development, whether measured categorically as language delay or continuously as a dimensional language score, aggregates in unaffected toddlers at elevated genetic risk of ASD. The result was particularly robust for receptive language, which displayed an effect of ASD risk on language delay as well as continuous language scores. Analyses in IBIS also confirmed that the effect of ASD-risk status on language was significant when controlling for sociodemographic factors known to be associated with language. The positive associations observed between 24-month language and female sex, maternal education, and income, are in agreement with the existing literature, illustrating the representativeness of the IBIS sample.
The language differences observed in HR-noASD toddlers fulfill several previously elaborated criteria for endophenotypes. First, an IBIS sub-analysis showed lower indices of language function, a heritable ability [9], in high-risk siblings with ASD versus HR-noASD siblings, satisfying the criterion of co-segregation (i.e., increased co-inheritance) in affected family members. As stated above, lower language abilities also aggregated in HR-noASD siblings compared to LR-noASD siblings, with more frequent language delay and lower mean language scores reflecting a pathological shift in underlying score distributions across the HR-noASD group. These findings affirm that decrements in early language function are associated with increased genetic liability for ASD and represent ASD endophenotypes. Prior studies reporting enhanced signal detection of ASD-associated genetic variants when language is incorporated in phenotyping [87, 88] provide empirical support for language’s value added as an ASD endophenotype and for the ability of family studies to inform genetic investigations of ASD.
Our approach attempted to maximize detectable variation in language by focusing on a narrow age range characterized by rapid growth in foundational language skills. Our findings thus also suggest that genetic factors influencing early language development overlap with genetic risk for ASD. These results extend prior work from large family [89] and factor analytic studies [90, 91] linking language impairment in older children and adults with the occurrence of ASD. They also corroborate a recent meta-analysis showing lower language scores in high-risk toddler siblings, which included siblings with and without ASD [92]. We chose to present both the current meta-analysis and IBIS analyses here given that (1) this meta-analysis evaluated risk group differences involving not only language scores but also language delay, (2) the replication provided here confirms language differences in high-risk siblings even when restricting analyses to high-risk siblings without ASD, (3) the meta-analysis motivated more in-depth analyses in IBIS, and (4) the dual approaches attempted to address longstanding inconsistencies in the literature by providing more representative evidence for early language as an ASD endophenotype.
Receptive language appears more affected than expressive language in high-risk siblings
HR-noASD siblings were three to four times more likely to exhibit language delay than LR-noASD siblings, consistent with a prior retrospective report [21], and IBIS analyses showed that receptive language delay, but not expressive language delay, accounts for much of this difference. Discrepant findings for receptive and expressive language delay are consistent with a lower signal for the expressive language endophenotype, which could be detected based on continuous scores but not the more stringent and less sensitive categorical language delay variable. Given that early language delay may be associated with subsequent language deficits [10, 47] and that receptive language impairment in particular is associated with worse functional outcomes than expressive language impairment [93], developmental surveillance in high-risk siblings with language delays may be especially important to capitalize on opportunities for intervention [94].
Summary effect sizes from the meta-analysis were also consistent with a larger effect of ASD-risk status for receptive language, and confidence intervals for receptive and expressive language were almost entirely non-overlapping (Fig. 1). This pattern parallels the preponderance of greater receptive versus expressive language deficits reported in children with ASD [95,96,97,98], as anticipated for an ASD endophenotype. Previous work has suggested that this discrepancy could reflect atypical mechanisms of language acquisition in ASD, whereby, in contrast to typical development, word comprehension appears less advanced than word production [97, 98]. While underlying mechanisms for this profile remain a question for future research, our findings reveal that at the group level, an analogous discrepancy in receptive and expressive language occurs in the context of ASD risk alone, without the elevated social deficits characteristic of ASD. These results also imply that the specific aspect of language measured may affect the ability to ascertain genetic associations between ASD and language and that some of the inconsistency in the literature could be resolved by distinguishing receptive and expressive language abilities.
One additional consideration regarding the distinct effects for receptive versus expressive language is the context of the assessment. Because the MSEL involves an interaction between an examiner and a child, the child’s social responsiveness, in itself a potential indicator of genetic liability for ASD, could impact his/her performance. It is possible that the evaluation of receptive language, which involves gauging a child’s response to an examiner’s prompt, may be more influenced by a child’s social responsiveness than expressive language, which may entail more self-motivated language output. While analyses in IBIS mitigate this concern by showing a similar relationship of ADOS social affect scores to both receptive and expressive language (Table 4), the persistent effect for ASD risk group in these models also implies the existence of shared genetic risk factors for ASD and language which are not associated with social performance. Thus, language assessments that reduce embedded social demands, for example, by using a psychophysiological measure such as auditory ERP to index language ability, rather than an interpersonal response, could be important for refining informative language endophenotypes of ASD.
Specificity of the language endophenotype differs for receptive and expressive language
To explore the specificity of language endophenotypes, we tested the impact of nonverbal cognitive development on the relationship between risk status and quantitative language function. This point is of particular interest in the high-risk group, since a higher frequency of general cognitive deficits has been observed in family members of individuals with ASD [28, 89], and slightly lower mean cognitive scores were observed in IBIS and other infant sibling samples [99]. As expected, nonverbal cognition did contribute to variation in receptive and expressive language. For receptive language, a significant effect of risk status persisted even with the addition of nonverbal cognition to the model, indicating that the effect of risk status on receptive language is relatively specific. For expressive language, however, the effect of ASD-risk status was no longer significant when accounting for nonverbal cognition, in line with previously described interrelationships between general cognitive abilities and language [84,85,86]. These findings further support separating receptive and expressive language in behavioral genetic analyses and imply that therapies targeting domain-general abilities may improve language in children with genetic liability for ASD, as shown in emerging work for several populations with language impairment [100,101,102,103].
Early social and language abilities are dissociated in high-risk toddlers without ASD
Because disrupted social and language development co-occur in ASD, we evaluated the relationship between language and autistic social deficits, based on the social affect score on the ADOS. As mentioned previously, the contribution of ASD risk status to both receptive and expressive language remained significant even when including social affect scores in the regression models. This suggests that heritable factors influencing early language and core autistic social features in the HR-noASD group are partially independent and that language deficits are unlikely to be purely secondary to ASD-related social deficits.
These analyses additionally revealed an interaction between risk group and social affect score, in which LR-noASD and HR-noASD siblings exhibited distinct relationships between language and social deficits. First, LR-noASD siblings displayed a negative relationship between levels of ASD-related social deficits and language. This concurred with a previously reported negative correlation between autistic traits and language in a general population toddler sample [104], confirming that the social affect score captured adequate variation in the low-risk group. In contrast, HR-noASD siblings showed no correlation between language and social domains. Like LR-noASD siblings, however, they showed moderate correlations between language and nonverbal cognition, reducing the likelihood that the dissociation of language and social domains was an epiphenomenon of broadly altered development.
A dissociation of early social and language abilities is reminiscent of the phenotype for the prior diagnosis of Asperger’s Disorder—in that case, early language development was relatively intact in the presence of autistic social impairment. At the same time, recent studies of older high-risk individuals without ASD have revealed an association of greater social deficits with lower language competency [35, 105]. In sum, while our findings show that genetic liability for ASD tracks with early language deficits, heritable factors for language and autistic social symptoms appear partially independent, and these phenotypes may not show steady covariation throughout development in HR-noASD siblings. Possible explanations include that some language-related social abilities are not measured by the ADOS, that language deficits may precede some ASD-related social deficits, or that, as has been described for specific language impairment, longstanding limitations in communication could reify a relationship between language and social function over time [106]. Although language impairment is no longer considered a core diagnostic feature of ASD in DSM-5 [107], behavioral genetic studies of trajectories of language development in conjunction with social development remain important to maximize the discovery of genetic contributors to ASD.
Limitations
While these findings support ASD-related language endophenotypes at an early and prolific stage of language development, we note that associating language differences in HR-noASD siblings with specific genetic risk factors is required for direct confirmation of these endophenotypes. The results are also cross-sectional and the relationship of these early language differences to later outcomes warrant future study. Other work has reported quantitative language differences in high-risk infants [83], but studies of older HR-noASD siblings are mixed regarding the stability of language differences [22, 28, 108, 109], and further investigation is needed to determine the continuity of language deficits in this group. Resolution of language deficits in HR-noASD siblings would suggest that manifestations of genetic liability for ASD could occur transiently as part of a developmental process like language acquisition, consistent with the possibility of “state-dependent” developmental endophenotypes.
Although ASD diagnoses are generally stable by age 24 months [54,55,56,57], with recent work showing 82.3% positive predictive value from 24 to 36 months [110], there is also variation in the early development of ASD [110,111,112], which could affect the composition of the HR-noASD group and consequently, the magnitude of risk-related differences. We additionally acknowledge that in keeping with the infant sibling study design, control groups were comprised of low-risk participants, who in some cases exhibited relatively elevated mean standardized language scores, maternal education, and socioeconomic status. Similar characteristics were also observed in some of the HR-noASD samples as well, suggesting possible enrollment bias, as frequently occurs in studies requiring high participant engagement. However, this design allowed analyses of extensive developmental assessments generally unavailable in non-clinical samples, and the similar prevalence of language delay for pooled LR-noASD siblings and the general population, 5.2% versus 6% [113], supports the comparability of LR-noASD siblings. Finally, larger, more diverse samples than those presented here would be necessary to address potential heterogeneity and subtyping (e.g., simplex versus multiplex families); nevertheless, it is notable that the signal for decreased language function in HR-noASD siblings was appreciable in spite of this known heterogeneity.
Conclusions
The detection of increased language delay in HR-noASD toddlers, both in existing literature and IBIS data, highlights the capacity to reliably measure heritable, clinically relevant markers of ASD risk during early development. At a practical level, the consistency of the findings across samples supports the utility of incorporating language metrics into early assessments, particularly for children at familial risk of ASD. Further characterization of the nature and continuity of language deficits in larger samples of ASD probands and their unaffected siblings is warranted (1) to refine genetically informative language phenotypes, taking into account receptive and expressive language, as well as potential contributions of social responsiveness, and (2) to resolve how deviations in early language development correspond to later ASD-related outcomes, e.g., pragmatic language deficits, which also occur more frequently in HR-noASD siblings [114]. Such studies would provide a valuable opportunity to clarify the heterogeneity of the genetic architecture ASD as well as the role and timing of language-based interventions.
Abbreviations
- ADOS:
-
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule
- ASD:
-
Autism spectrum disorder
- BSRC:
-
Baby Siblings Research Consortium
- CNTNAP:
-
Contactin-associated protein
- DSM:
-
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Psychiatric Disorders
- HR-noASD:
-
High-risk siblings without autism spectrum disorder
- IBIS:
-
Infant Brain Imaging Study
- LR-noASD:
-
Low-risk siblings without autism spectrum disorder
- MRI:
-
Magnetic resonance imaging
- MSEL:
-
Mullen Scales of Early Learning
- NOS:
-
Not otherwise specified
References
Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:636–45.
Lenzenweger MF. Thinking clearly about the endophenotype-intermediate phenotype-biomarker distinctions in developmental psychopathology research. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25:1347–57.
Magiati I, Tay XW, Howlin P. Cognitive, language, social and behavioural outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review of longitudinal follow-up studies in adulthood. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34:73–86.
Kjelgaard MM, Tager-Flusberg H. An investigation of language impairment in autism: implications for genetic subgroups. Lang Cogn Process. 2001;16:287–308.
Williams D, Botting N, Boucher J. Language in autism and specific language impairment: where are the links? Psychol Bull. 2008;134:944–63.
Bishop DV, Laws G, Adams C, Norbury CF. High heritability of speech and language impairments in 6-year-old twins demonstrated using parent and teacher report. Behav Genet. 2006;36:173–84.
Bishop DV, North T, Donlan C. Genetic basis of specific language impairment: evidence from a twin study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1995;37:56–71.
Lewis BA, Thompson LA. A study of developmental speech and language disorders in twins. J Speech Hear Res. 1992;35:1086–94.
Stromswold K. The heritability of language: a review and metaanalysis of twin, adoption, and linkage studies. Language. 2001;77:647–723.
Dale PS, Price TS, Bishop DV, Plomin R. Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46:544–60.
Viding E, Spinath FM, Price TS, Bishop DV, Dale PS, Plomin R. Genetic and environmental influence on language impairment in 4-year-old same-sex and opposite-sex twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45:315–25.
Lyall K, Constantino JN, Weisskopf MG, Roberts AL, Ascherio A, Santangelo SL. Parental social responsiveness and risk of autism spectrum disorder in offspring. JAMA Psychiat. 2014;71:936–42.
Lowe JK, Werling DM, Constantino JN, Cantor RM, Geschwind DH. Social responsiveness, an autism endophenotype: genomewide significant linkage to two regions on chromosome 8. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172:266–75.
Dale PS, Simonoff E, Bishop DV, Eley TC, Oliver B, Price TS, et al. Genetic influence on language delay in two-year-old children. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:324–8.
Bishop DV, Bishop SJ, Bright P, James C, Delaney T, Tallal P. Different origin of auditory and phonological processing problems in children with language impairment: evidence from a twin study. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42:155–68.
Cox A, Rutter M, Newman S, Bartak L. A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder. II. Parental characteristics. Br J Psychiatry. 1975;126:146–59.
Bailey A, Palferman S, Heavey L, Le Couteur A. Autism: the phenotype in relatives. J Autism Dev Disord. 1998;28:369–92.
Le Couteur A, Bailey A, Goode S, Pickles A, Robertson S, Gottesman I, et al. A broader phenotype of autism: the clinical spectrum in twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996;37:785–801.
Piven J, Palmer P, Jacobi D, Childress D, Arndt S. Broader autism phenotype: evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:185–90.
Bolton P, Macdonald H, Pickles A, Rios P, Goode S, Crowson M, et al. A case-control family history study of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994;35:877–900.
Constantino JN, Zhang Y, Frazier T, Abbacchi AM, Law P. Sibling recurrence and the genetic epidemiology of autism. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:1349–56.
Gamliel I, Yirmiya N, Jaffe DH, Manor O, Sigman M. Developmental trajectories in siblings of children with autism: cognition and language from 4 months to 7 years. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39:1131–44.
Chuthapisith J, Ruangdaraganon N, Sombuntham T, Roongpraiwan R. Language development among the siblings of children with autistic spectrum disorder. Autism. 2007;11:149–60.
LeBarton ES, Iverson JM. Gesture development in toddlers with an older sibling with autism. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2016;51:18–30.
Silverman JM, Smith CJ, Schmeidler J, Hollander E, Lawlor BA, Fitzgerald M, et al. Symptom domains in autism and related conditions: evidence for familiality. Am J Med Genet. 2002;114:64–73.
Pilowsky T, Yirmiya N, Shalev RS, Gross-Tsur V. Language abilities of siblings of children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:914–25.
Whitehouse AJ, Barry JG, Bishop DV. The broader language phenotype of autism: a comparison with specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48:822–30.
Charman T, Young GS, Brian J, Carter A, Carver LJ, Chawarska K, et al. Non-ASD outcomes at 36 months in siblings at familial risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): a baby siblings research consortium (BSRC) study. Autism Res. 2017;10:169–78.
Levy Y, Bar-Yuda C. Language performance in siblings of nonverbal children with autism. Autism. 2011;15:341–54.
Tomblin JB, Hafeman LL, O'Brien M. Autism and autism risk in siblings of children with specific language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2003;38:235–50.
Pickles A, St Clair MC, Conti-Ramsden G. Communication and social deficits in relatives of individuals with SLI and relatives of individuals with ASD. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43:156–67.
Mouridsen SE, Hauschild KM. Autism spectrum disorders in siblings of children with a developmental language disorder. Logoped Phoniatr Vocol. 2011;36:145–9.
Plumet MH, Goldblum MC, Leboyer M. Verbal skills in relatives of autistic females. Cortex. 1995;31:723–33.
Dworzynski K, Ronald A, Hayiou-Thomas M, Rijsdijk F, Happe F, Bolton PF, et al. Aetiological relationship between language performance and autistic-like traits in childhood: a twin study. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2007;42:273–92.
Frazier TW, Youngstrom EA, Hardan AY, Georgiades S, Constantino JN, Eng C. Quantitative autism symptom patterns recapitulate differential mechanisms of genetic transmission in single and multiple incidence families. Mol Autism. 2015;6:58.
Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, et al. A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. N Engl J Med. 2008;359:2337–45.
Alarcon M, Abrahams BS, Stone JL, Duvall JA, Perederiy JV, Bomar JM, et al. Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. Am J Hum Genet. 2008;82:150–9.
Arking DE, Cutler DJ, Brune CW, Teslovich TM, West K, Ikeda M, et al. A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am J Hum Genet. 2008;82:160–4.
Alarcon M, Cantor RM, Liu J, Gilliam TC, Geschwind DH. Autism Genetic Research Exchange. Evidence for a language quantitative trait locus on chromosome 7q in multiplex autism families. Am J Hum Genet. 2002;70:60–71.
Alarcon M, Yonan AL, Gilliam TC, Cantor RM, Geschwind DH. Quantitative genome scan and ordered-subsets analysis of autism endophenotypes support language QTLs. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:747–57.
Warburton P, Baird G, Chen W, Morris K, Jacobs BW, Hodgson S, et al. Support for linkage of autism and specific language impairment to 7q3 from two chromosome rearrangements involving band 7q31. Am J Med Genet. 2000;96:228–34.
Bartlett CW, Flax JF, Fermano Z, Hare A, Hou L, Petrill SA, et al. Gene x gene interaction in shared etiology of autism and specific language impairment. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72:692–9.
Ashley-Koch A, Wolpert CM, Menold MM, Zaeem L, Basu S, Donnelly SL, et al. Genetic studies of autistic disorder and chromosome 7. Genomics. 1999;61:227–36.
Ellis Weismer S, Lord C, Esler A. Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:259–1273.
Mitchell S, Brian J, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Szatmari P, Smith I, et al. Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27:S69–78.
Wetherby AM, Woods J, Allen L, Cleary J, Dickinson H, Lord C. Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:473–93.
Rescorla L. Age 13 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2009;52:16-30. Dev Disabil Res Rev 2005;48:459–472.
Rescorla L. Late talkers: do good predictors of outcome exist? Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2011;17:141–50.
Duff FJ, Reen G, Plunkett K, Nation K. Do infant vocabulary skills predict school-age language and literacy outcomes? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56:848–56.
Preston JL, Frost SJ, Mencl WE, Fulbright RK, Landi N, Grigorenko E, et al. Early and late talkers: school-age language, literacy and neurolinguistic differences. Brain. 2010;133:2185–95.
Estes A, Zwaigenbaum L, Gu H, St John T, Paterson S, Elison JT, et al. Behavioral, cognitive, and adaptive development in infants with autism spectrum disorder in the first 2 years of life. J Neurodev Disord. 2015;7:24.
Falagas ME, Pitsouni EI, Malietzis GA, Pappas G. Comparison of PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, and Google Scholar: strengths and weaknesses. FASEB J. 2008;22:338–42.
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.
Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson SE, Brian J, Smith IM, Roberts W, Szatmari P, et al. Stability of diagnostic assessment for autism spectrum disorder between 18 and 36 months in a high-risk cohort. Autism Res. 2016;9:790–800.
Lord C, Risi S, DiLavore PS, Shulman C, Thurm A, Pickles A. Autism from 2 to 9 years of age. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:694–701.
Guthrie W, Swineford LB, Nottke C, Wetherby AM. Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54:582–90.
Cox A, Klein K, Charman T, Baird G, Baron-Cohen S, Swettenham J, et al. Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42 months of age: stability of clinical and ADI-R diagnosis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;40:719–32.
Rogers SJ. What are infant siblings teaching us about autism in infancy? Autism Res. 2009;2:125–37.
Ozonoff S, Iosif AM, Baguio F, Cook IC, Hill MM, Hutman T, Rogers SJ, Rozga A, Sangha S, Sigman M, Steinfeld MB, Young GS. A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:256–66.
Schmidt FL, Hunter JE. General technical issues in meta-analysis. In: Methods of meta-analysis: correcting error and bias in research findings. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 2004. p. 371–433.
Mullen E. Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: Guidance Service Publishing; 1995.
Hudry K, Chandler S, Bedford R, Pasco G, Gliga T, Elsabbagh M, et al. Early language profiles in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:154–67.
Ference J, Curtin S. Attention to lexical stress and early vocabulary growth in 5-month-olds at risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;116(4):891–903.
Ference J, Curtin S. The ability to map differentially stressed labels to objects predicts language development at 24 months in 12-month-olds at high risk for autism. Infancy. 2015;20:242–62.
Fenson L, Marchman VA, Thal DJ, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E. MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual. 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 2006.
Messinger DS, Young GS, Webb SJ, Ozonoff S, Bryson SE, Carter A, et al. Early sex differences are not autism-specific: a baby siblings research consortium (BSRC) study. Mol Autism. 2015;6:32.
Ozonoff S, Young GS, Belding A, Hill M, Hill A, Hutman T, et al. The broader autism phenotype in infancy: when does it emerge? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53:398–407.
Finch KH, Seery AM, Talbott MR, Nelson CA, Tager-Flusberg H. Lateralization of ERPs to speech and handedness in the early development of autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord. 2017;9:4.
Rutter M, LeCouteur A, Lord C. Autism diagnostic interview-revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Service; 2003.
Swanson MR, Shen MD, Wolff JJ, Elison JT, Emerson R, Styner M, Hazlett H, Truong K, Watson L, Paterson S, Marrus N, Botteron K, Pandey J, Schultz RT, Dager S, Zwaigenbaum L, Estes AM, Piven J, for the IBIS Network+. Subcortical brain and behavior phenotypes differentiate infants with autism versus language delay. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroim. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.07.007.
Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, Messinger D, Yirmiya N, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics. 2011;128:e488–95.
Landa R, Garrett-Mayer E. Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: a prospective study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:629–38.
Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2000.
Gotham K, Risi S, Pickles A, Lord C. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:613–27.
Hus V, Gotham K, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS domain scores: separating severity of social affect and restricted and repetitive behaviors. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:2400–12.
Mantel N, Haenszel W. Statistical aspects of the analysis of data from retrospective studies of disease. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1959;22:719–48.
Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical meta-analysis. Thosand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2001.
JPT H, Green S, editors. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]: The Cochrane Collaboration; 2011. Available from www.handbook.cochrane.org. Accessed June 20, 2018
Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E, Thal DJ, Pethick SJ. Variability in early communicative development. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1994;59:1–173 discussion 174-85.
Huttenlocher J, Haight W, Bryk A, Seltzer M, Lyons T. Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and sex. Dev Psychol. 1991;27:236–48.
Hoff-Ginsberg E. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children's language experience and language development. Appl Psycholinguist. 1998;19:603–29.
Dollaghan CA, Campbell TF, Paradise JL, Feldman HM, Janosky JE, Pitcairn DN, et al. Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42:1432–43.
Pungello EP, Iruka IU, Dotterer AM, Mills-Koonce R, Reznick JS. The effects of socioeconomic status, race, and parenting on language development in early childhood. Dev Psychol. 2009;45:544–57.
Price TS, Eley TC, Dale PS, Stevenson J, Saudino K, Plomin R. Genetic and environmental covariation between verbal and nonverbal cognitive development in infancy. Child Dev. 2000;71:948–59.
Libertus K, Violi DA. Sit to talk: relation between motor skills and language development in infancy. Front Psychol. 2016;7:475.
Luyster RJ, Kadlec MB, Carter A, Tager-Flusberg H. Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:1426–38.
Barlett CW, Hou L, Flax JF, Hare A, Cheong SY, Fermano Z, et al. A genome scan for loci shared by autism spectrum disorder and language impairment. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171:72–81.
Bradford Y, Haines J, Hutcheson H, Gardiner M, Braun T, Sheffield V, et al. Incorporating language phenotypes strengthens evidence of linkage to autism. Am J Med Genet. 2001;8:539–57.
Szatmari P, Merette C, Emond C, Zwaigenbaum L, Jones MB, Maziade M, et al. Decomposing the autism phenotype into familial dimensions. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B:3–9.
Steer CD, Golding J, Bolton PF. Traits contributing to the autistic spectrum. PLoS One. 2010;5:e12633.
Pandolfi V, Magyar CI, Dill CA. Constructs assessed by the GARS-2: factor analysis of data from the standardization sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:1118–30.
Garrido D, Petrova D, Watson LR, Garcia-Retamero R, Carballo G. Language and motor skills in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analytic review. Autism Res. 2017;10:1737–50.
Beitchman JH, Wilson B, Brownlie EB, Walters H, Inglis A, Lancee W. Long-term consistency in speech/language profiles: II. Behavioral, emotional, and social outcomes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35:815–25.
Roberts MY, Kaiser AP. Early intervention for toddlers with language delays: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2015;135:686–93.
Bartak L, Rutter M, Cox A. A comparative study of infantile autism and specific development receptive language disorder. I. The children. Br J Psychiatry. 1975;126:127–45.
Tsai LY, Beisler JM. Research in infantile autism: a methodological problem in using language comprehension as the basis for selecting matched controls. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1984;23:700–3.
Charman T, Drew A, Baird C, Baird G. Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). J Child Lang. 2003;30:213–36.
Hudry K, Leadbitter K, Temple K, Slonims V, McConachie H, Aldred C, Howlin P, Charman T, Pact Consortium. Preschoolers with autism show greater impairment in receptive compared with expressive language abilities. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2010;45:681–90.
Messinger D, Young GS, Ozonoff S, Dobkins K, Carter A, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Beyond autism: a baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. J Am Acad Child Adoles Psychiatry. 2013;52:300–8 e1.
Dawson G, Rogers S, Munson J, Smith M, Winter J, Greenson J, et al. Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: the Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics. 2010;125:e17–23.
Sun IYI, Varanda CA, Fernandes FD. Stimulation of executive functions as part of the language intervention process in children with autism spectrum disorder. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2017;69:78–83.
van der Schuit M, Segers E, van Balkom H, Verhoeven L. Early language intervention for children with intellectual disabilities: a neurocognitive perspective. Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32:705–12.
Ebert KD, Kohnert K. (2009) Non-linguistic cognitive treatment for primary language impairment. Clin Linguist Phon. 2009;23:647–66.
Marrus N, Glowinski AL, Jacob T, Klin A, Jones W, Drain CE, et al. Rapid video-referenced ratings of reciprocal social behavior in toddlers: a twin study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56:1338–46.
Howlin P, Moss P, Savage S, Bolton P, Rutter M. Outcomes in adult life among siblings of individuals with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:707–18.
Howlin P, Mawhood L, Rutter M. Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—a follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:561–78.
DSM-5 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
Ben-Yizhak N, Yirmiya N, Seidman I, Alon R, Lord C, Sigman M. Pragmatic language and school related linguistic abilities in siblings of children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:750–60.
Miller M, Iosif AM, Young GS, Hill M, Phelps Hanzel E, Hutman T, et al. School-age outcomes of infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2016;9:632–42.
Ozonoff S, Young GS, Landa RJ, Brian J, Bryson S, Charman T, et al. Diagnostic stability in young children at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a baby siblings research consortium study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56:988–98.
Brian J, Bryson SE, Smith IM, Roberts W, Roncadin C, Szatmari P, Zwaigenbaum L. Stability and change in autism spectrum disorder diagnosis from age 3 to middle childhood in a high-risk sibling cohort. Autism. 2016;20:888–92.
Shephard E, Milosavljevic B, Pasco G, Jones EJ, Gliga T, Happé F, Johnson MH, Charman T, BASIS Team. Mid-childhood outcomes of infant siblings at familial high-risk of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2017;10:546–57.
Law J, Boyle J, Harris F, Harkness A, Nye C. Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: findings from a systematic review of the literature. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2000;35:165–88.
Miller M, Young GS, Hutman T, Johnson S, Schwichtenberg AJ, Ozonoff S. Early pragmatic language difficulties in siblings of children with autism: implications for DSM-5 social communication disorder? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56:774–81.
Iverson JM, Wozniak RH. Variation in vocal-motor development in infant siblings of children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:158–70.
Gamliel I, Yirmiya N, Sigman M. The development of young siblings of children with autism from 4 to 54 months. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:171–83.
Toth K, Dawson G, Meltzoff AN, Greenson J, Fein D. Early social, imitation, play, and language abilities of young non-autistic siblings of children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:145–57.
Stone WL, McMahon CR, Yoder PJ, Walden TA. Early social-communicative and cognitive development of younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161:384–90.
Paul R, Fuerst Y, Ramsay G, Chawarska K, Klin A. Out of the mouths of babes: vocal production in infant siblings of children with ASD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:588–98.
Curtin S, Vouloumanos A. Speech preference is associated with autistic-like behavior in 18-months-olds at risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43:2114–20.
Acknowledgements
We thank the families and children for their time and participation.
We thank Penelope Kostopoulos and Leigh MacIntyre for managing the IBIS database. We thank Susan Vugteveen and Alicia Rocca for helping to proof and assemble the manuscript.
The Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS) Network is an NIH funded Autism Centers of Excellence project and consists of a consortium of eight universities in the USA and Canada. Clinical sites: University of North Carolina: J. Piven (IBIS Network PI), H.C. Hazlett, C. Chappell; University of Washington: S. Dager, A. Estes, D. Shaw; Washington University: K. Botteron, R. McKinstry, J. Constantino, J. Pruett; The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia: R.T. Schultz, S. Paterson; University of Alberta: L. Zwaigenbaum; University of Minnesota: J. Elison; Data Coordinating Center: Montreal Neurological Institute: A.C. Evans, D.L. Collins, G.B. Pike, V. Fonov, P. Kostopoulos; S. Das; Image Processing Core: New York University: G. Gerig; University of North Carolina: M. Styner; Statistical Analysis Core: University of North Carolina: H. Gu.
Funding
This study was supported by National Institutes of Health Autism Center of Excellence R01 grant (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, #HD055741 to J.P.), Autism Speaks (#6020 to J.P.), the Simons Foundation (grant number #140209 to J.P.), the National Institute of Mental Health (K08 MH112891 to N.M.), the Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Centers at Washington University (National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Child Health and Human Development U54 HD087011 to J.N.C.). Children's Hospital of Pennsylvania/University of Pennsylvania (National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Child Health and Human Development U54 HD086984 to R.T.S.), and the University of North Carolina (National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Child Health and Human Development U54 HD079124 to J.P.).
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used for the current study are available from senior author J.P. on reasonable request.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Consortia
Contributions
NM and JNC were the major contributors in writing the manuscript. NM, LPH, JNC, and J.P. conceptualized and designed the study. NM and LPH analyzed the data. J.P., HCH, KNB, A.E., SD, RTS, SJP, JTE, JJW, and LZ contributed to the data acquisition and curation. J.P., NM, and JNC contributed to acquisition of funding support. All authors have been involved in interpreting the findings and critically revising the manuscript. All authors have given final approval for publication. All authors agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This study was approved by the institutional review boards at all sites of data acquisition for the study. Informed consent was obtained from the parents of the participants.
Consent for publication
Not applicable
Competing interests
Dr. John Constantino received royalties from the Western Psychological Services for the Social Responsiveness Scale.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Additional file
Additional file 1:
Table S1. Study Characteristics of Publications in Secondary Meta-analysis. Table S2. Group Moderation of Relationships between Social Performance and Language Using ADOS Calibrated Severity Social Affect Scores. Figure S1. Secondary Meta-analysis of Language Scores in High-risk Siblings without ASD. Figure S2. Social Affect Scores at 24 months of Age in IBIS Infant Siblings. (DOCX 224 kb)
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
About this article
Cite this article
Marrus, N., Hall, L.P., Paterson, S.J. et al. Language delay aggregates in toddler siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodevelop Disord 10, 29 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-018-9247-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-018-9247-8