Skip to main content

Reduced conditioned fear response in mice that lack Dlx1 and show subtype-specific loss of interneurons

Abstract

The inhibitory GABAergic system has been implicated in multiple neuropsychiatric diseases such as schizophrenia and autism. The Dlx homeobox transcription factor family is essential for development and function of GABAergic interneurons. Mice lacking the Dlx1 gene have postnatal subtype-specific loss of interneurons and reduced IPSCs in their cortex and hippocampus. To ascertain consequences of these changes in the GABAergic system, we performed a battery of behavioral assays on the Dlx1 mutant mice, including zero maze, open field, locomotor activity, food intake, rotarod, tail suspension, fear conditioning assays (context and trace), prepulse inhibition, and working memory related tasks (spontaneous alteration task and spatial working memory task). Dlx1 mutant mice displayed elevated activity levels in open field, locomotor activity, and tail suspension tests. These mice also showed deficits in contextual and trace fear conditioning, and possibly in prepulse inhibition. Their learning deficits were not global, as the mutant mice did not differ from the wild-type controls in tests of working memory. Our findings demonstrate a critical role for the Dlx1 gene, and likely the subclasses of interneurons that are affected by the lack of this gene, in behavioral inhibition and associative fear learning. These observations support the involvement of particular components of the GABAergic system in specific behavioral phenotypes related to complex neuropsychiatric diseases.

Introduction

GABAergic inhibitory interneurons are vital for cortical development and function [1, 2]. They can be distinguished from pyramidal cells by their smooth or sparsely spiny dendrites and locally projecting axons. Interneurons are vastly heterogeneous in their morphological, physiological, molecular and synaptic characteristics, despite the fact that this cell population comprises only 20–30% of cortical neurons. Interneuron deficits are implicated in neuropsychiatric disorders including schizophrenia [3, 4], autism [5, 6], anxiety [7, 8], and mood disorders [911]. However, our understanding of the development and function of these cells is limited.

The family of Dlx homeobox transcription factors regulates the development of inhibitory interneurons; members of this family of genes, namely Dlx1, Dlx2, Dlx5, and Dlx6, control differentiation of GABAergic neurons in the basal ganglia and cerebral cortex [1217]. Dlx1 is essential for maintaining the functional longevity of interneurons in the adult brain: homozygous Dlx1 knockout mice (Dlx1−/−) show partial loss of specific subsets of postnatal neocortical and hippocampal interneurons as a result of apoptosis, along with functional onset of epilepsy [18].

Given the importance of the Dlx1 gene in interneuron development and survival, Dlx1 knockout mice provide an opportunity to probe whether loss of these neurons results in behavioral phenotypes that present in neuropsychiatric illnesses. Many behavioral phenotypes have been reported in transgenic and knockout mice, such as learning and memory impairments, feeding disorders, aberrant social, reproductive, parental behaviors, behaviors relevant to human anxiety, depression, schizophrenia, autism, drug addiction, and many others [1921]. These studies have made important contributions to our knowledge of genetic mechanisms underlying abnormal behaviors in the neuropsychiatric disorders.

In this paper, we report the results of a battery of tests aimed at characterizing behavioral deficits in Dlx1 mutant mice. We find that the mutant mice appear normal in their food intake, and in their performance on zero maze and on rotarod tasks; however, they exhibit hyperactivity in open field, in locomotor activity, and in tail suspension tests. Furthermore, they show significantly reduced conditioned fear but no deficits in spontaneous alteration task and spatial working memory. Despite the broad expression of Dlx1 in forebrain GABAergic neurons, Dlx1−/− mutants exhibit relatively specific behavioral phenotypes. This analysis suggests that defects in Dlx1 function could contribute to human neuropsychiatric disorders of cognition and emotion.

Materials and methods

Animals

Dlx1 knockout (Dlx1−/−), heterozygous (Dlx1+/−) and wild-type (Dlx1+/+) mice [22], all age-matched at the time of testing, were maintained on a C57BL/6 J × CD1 background [18]. Wild-type C57BL/6 J mice (The Jackson Laboratory, Bar Harbor, Maine, USA) were used as an additional comparison group for spontaneous alternation and spatial working memory tasks. Mouse colonies were maintained at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT) or at the University of California, San Francisco (UCSF), in accordance with National Institutes of Health, MIT, and UCSF guidelines for care and use of lab animals.

General behavioral assays were performed on one set of mice (aged 3–6 months), which were all males from three genotype groups (Dlx1−/−, Dlx1+/− and Dlx1+/+), in the following testing order: zero maze, open field, locomotor activity, food intake, rotarod, and finally tail suspension. We did not perform fear conditioning, prepulse inhibition, or working memory tasks on this set of mice. Mice were group-housed (4–6 mice per cage) in standard polycarbonate mouse cages (29 × 18.5 × 13 cm). They had free access to food and water, and were maintained in a 12 hour light/dark cycle. Between subjects, the testing apparatus was cleaned with a 0.25% bleach solution, wiped down with water, and dried. With the exception of tests requiring the monitoring of photobeam activity, all animals were tested during the light cycle. Experimental conditions were counterbalanced by genotype.

Multiple sets of the mice (age ≥ 2 months) were tested for assays in the following order: visual behavioral assays and then fear conditioning (context only, or context and trace). Some sets of these mice were finally tested with prepulse inhibition (aged ≥ 3 months).

A different set of mice (all males) were tested with the spontaneous alteration task and spatial working memory task. Dlx1 mice (Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1−/−) tested on these tasks were 11–15 weeks old and group-housed; C57BL/6 J mice used as age matched controls were 12–15 weeks old and group-housed. All mice were tested on the spontaneous alteration task first and then on the spatial working memory task 1–2 weeks later. These mice were not tested on any other behavioral assays reported in this manuscript.

For all behavioral tests, investigators were blind to the genotypes. Approximately 3 min before each assay, the animal was removed from its home cage and placed in a clean holding cage for transfer; an exception to this was the tasks requiring photobeam activity monitoring, where animals were placed directly into the test chambers.

Zero maze

A zero maze (34 cm inner diameter, 46 cm outer diameter, placed 40 cm off the ground on 4 braced legs) was used as previously described [23]. Mice were removed and placed in the center of one of the closed quadrants. The test lasted 6 min. Latency in entering (all four paws) an open quadrant, time spent in the open quadrants, and number of closed-to-open quadrant transitions was live-scored in a blinded fashion by a remote observer.

Open field

A 4-unit open field consisting of a white Kydex box divided into four separate 50 × 50 × 38 cm chambers was used, allowing four animals to be tested concurrently. A video camera was mounted directly above the chambers to monitor the animals’ activity and movements. Four mice were simultaneously placed into the open field chambers. A video tracking system (Poly-track, San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA) was used to measure distance traveled in 5 min monitoring sessions. Assignments to the four chambers were counterbalanced by genotype. Mouse activity within 7 cm of the chamber walls was defined as occurring within a peripheral zone; activity further away from the walls was considered to occur within a center zone.

Locomotor activity

Animals (Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1−/− mice; Dlx1+/− mice were not tested due to limited availability of activity cages) were housed individually in larger cages (48 × 27 × 13 cm) with bedding, food and water, under a 12-hour light/dark cycle (lights on 7 AM). To assess activity, we used a photobeam system (FlexField, San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA), which records horizontal locomotor activity (as monitored by a 4 × 8 array of infra-red photobeams). Data were collected for three days, 24 h each day, with animals placed in the system at noon on day 1.

Food intake

During the locomotor activity run described above, daily food (standard UCSF laboratory chow pellets) intake measures were made by weighing the food daily.

Rotarod

Motor coordination was assessed with an Accurotor rotarod machine (Accuscan Instruments, Columbus, Ohio, USA). The rotation rate was accelerated from zero to 50 rpm over 5 min. Four animals were tested concurrently in separate 11 cm-wide compartments, on a rod that was approximately 3 cm in diameter and was elevated 35 cm. Each animal was assessed over nine trials with 20 min inter-trial intervals (ITIs). For each trial, the latency to fall from the rod was recorded.

Tail suspension

Using adhesive tape, animals were suspended by the tail from a 1.2 cm diameter metal bar elevated 30–35 cm. When suspended, rodents either make “escape attempt” movements, or adopt a characteristic immobile posture. The total time for which the mouse was immobile was measured during a 6 min test period.

Fear conditioning

All mice were allowed to rest undisturbed in their transportable home cage at least seven days prior to the fear conditioning. To minimize occurrence of handling-induced seizures in the mutant mice [18], we habituated all animals for 3–5 sessions (~10 min in each session, once a day) prior to the day of the first experiment. In these habituation sessions, mice were acclimated to the room and handling of the experimenter. The experimenter wore gloves, habituated the mice to handling, allowing them to walk or run from hand to hand, until the mice showed no apparent signs of stress or fear.

An automated fear-conditioning system (TSE Systems, Bad Homburg, Germany) was used for fear conditioning. Some mice were tested with contextual fear conditioning only (without exposure to a light or tone); the others were tested with both contextual and trace fear conditioning. For each mouse from the Dlx1−/−, Dlx1+/− or Dlx1+/+ groups, training (Day 1) consisted of a 3 min exposure to the conditioning box (context). In the case of contextual fear conditioning only, this was followed by a foot shock (2 sec, 1.0 mA, constant current). In the trace conditioning sessions, the 3-minute exposure to the context was followed by a visual or auditory cue. In the case of visually cued fear conditioning, the cue was a 30 sec flickering light at 1 Hz, followed by a 15 sec trace interval, and then a foot shock (2 sec, 1.0 mA, constant current). Because a visual cue tends to be less effective in eliciting a defensive response than an auditory cue and often requires more conditioned stimulus (CS)—unconditioned stimulus (US) pairings to the light alone [24], the light-pulse-shock sequence was repeated twice before removing the mouse from the box (15 sec ITI). In the case of an auditory cue, after a 3 min exposure to the box, the mouse was subjected to the tone (30 sec, 10 kHz, 75 dB sound pressure level (SPL), 200 ms interval), a 15 sec trace interval, a foot shock (2 sec, 1.0 mA, constant current), and finally a 15 sec interval before removing it from the context. Given a positive correlation of shock intensity (ranging from 0.25 mA to 1.0 mA) with percent of freezing in mice [25], we used a shock intensity of 1.0 mA, which is at the higher end of this range, to ensure that both the wild-type and mutant mice would be conditioned with the foot shock.

The contextual memory test (Day 2) was performed 24 h after training by re-exposing the mice for 3 min to the conditioning context. Freezing, defined as a lack of movement except for heart rate and respiration, associated with a crouching posture, was assessed every 10 sec, for 180 sec, by a trained observer who was unaware of the genotypes. The number of observations indicating freezing was expressed as a percentage of the total number of observations. Baseline freezing for the contextual test refers to freezing (if any) on day 1 during the 3 min exposure to the context, before the tone and foot shock in the training session. We performed the contextual memory test alone, or combined it with the trace conditioning. The results did not differ in the two cases. We report the results on contextual fear conditioning that was carried out alone, i.e. without a visual or auditory cue paired to the shock.

The trace test was performed on day 3, 24 h after the contextual memory test. Mice were placed in a novel context. After a 30 s habituation period, a 180 s flickering light (1 Hz) or a 180 s tone (10 kHz, 75 dB SPL, 200 ms interval) was presented in the absence of a foot shock; freezing behavior was recorded every 10 sec, over 18 intervals (see above). Baseline freezing for the trace test refers to freezing (if any) on day 3 in the second context, before presenting the light or tone.

Prepulse inhibition

Mice were tested using an ASR-PRO1 acoustic startle reflex test apparatus (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA). Mice were acclimated to the testing room for 1 h prior to testing. For 2 min before start of trials, mice were acclimated to the apparatus. Trials were given at an interval of 3–8 s (randomized). Trials consisted of either a 40 ms startle stimulus alone (110 dB white noise) or a startle stimulus preceded 100 ms earlier by a 20 ms white noise prepulse at 8 dB, 12 dB, or 16 dB above background (60 dB white noise). Five trials for each stimulus configuration were recorded using Startle Reflex Software (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA).

Visual cliff test

Mice were placed in a 60 × 60 cm box with a clear Perspex base. A high-contrast grating was attached to the underside of one half of the box; the clear half of the box was placed such that it protruded from the table, revealing a drop to the floor of approximately 90 cm. Mice were placed in the center of the box and their behavior was monitored for 5 min, via a digital video camera mounted above the box. The amount of time spent in each half of the box was recorded. To confirm the role of the visual system in this task, the test was also performed under dark (red light) conditions.

Visual water maze test

Mice were tested in a circular tank (diameter 1.2 m) filled with opaque water. In the center of the target quadrant, a platform (11 × 11 cm) was submerged below the water surface, but with a visual cue (a cylinder with high-contrast gratings on the surface) above the water to guide the mouse. The swimming paths of the mice were recorded by a video camera, and analyzed off line by the Videomot 2 software (TSE Systems, Bad Homburg, Germany). Each mouse was placed into the maze from four random points of the tank, and allowed to search for the platform. The amount of time that each mouse took to find the platform was recorded and averaged for comparison.

Spontaneous alternation task (Y-maze)

Three open arms of an automated radial maze were used to form a Y-maze. Proximal visual cues were attached to the arms and to the hub of the maze to orient the mice in space. C57BL/6 J, Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1−/− mice were placed in the hub and allowed to traverse the three arms for 8 min. Based on the natural levels of activity in the maze, which peaked at about 2 min, the data were analyzed for the first 2 min, as well as for the entire 8 min of maze exploration. Alternation was considered to be ABC, BCA or CAB, etc, in any order or overlapping occurrence; but not ABA or BBC. The number of entries into each arm, the percent of alternation ((number of alternations / (total number of arm entries-2)) × 100), and the percent of repeat entries into the same arm were calculated.

Spatial working memory task (novel arm in a Y-maze)

For this task, the animal (a C57BL/6 J, Dlx1+/+ or Dlx1−/− mouse) first explored two randomly selected arms of the Y-maze for 5 min, followed by a 2 min ITI in which the mouse was confined to the center hub of the maze, with all maze arms closed. Next, all three Y-maze arms were opened, and the mouse was free to explore the entire maze for 5 min. The data from the retrieval phase were analyzed to determine if mice recognized the novel arm, expressed by enhanced exploration.

Two measures of novelty exploration/working memory were used to analyze the data from the retrieval phase of this assay: the number of entries into the Y-maze arms and the total amount of time spent in each of the arms.

Statistics

All p values reported in the results were obtained either from ANOVA for overall comparisons of three or more groups of mice (using genotype as the independent variable) except the repeated measures ANOVA described below, or from t-tests for comparisons between two groups of mice. Locomotor activity of Dlx1 mice was analyzed using repeated measures ANOVA with genotype as the between factor, and activity counts over time as the within factor.

Analysis of the prepulse inhibition assay was carried out using repeated measures ANOVA with genotype as the between group variable and prepulse intensity as the within group variable. Where a significant effect was seen between groups of mice, a post-hoc comparison was performed using the Tukey HSD test.

Statistical analysis of the spontaneous alternation task was performed using one-way ANOVA with genotype as the independent variable and number of entries, percent alternation, and percent repeated entries as dependent variables. Data from spatial working memory task were analyzed using repeated measures ANOVA, with genotype as the between variable, number of entries, and time spent in arms as within variables (separate ANOVA for each).

Results

General activity levels

Our general screen for behavioral phenotypes included the following assays performed on Dlx1−/−, Dlx1+/−, and Dlx1+/+ mice: zero maze, open field, locomotor activity, food intake, rotarod, and tail suspension.

Zero maze

No significant differences were detected among the three genotypes on the three measures: open quadrant time, open quadrant entries, and latency to enter open quadrant (data not shown; Dlx1−/−, n = 7; Dlx1+/−, n = 9; Dlx1+/+, n = 10).

Open field

Overlaid traces for the three genotype groups are presented in Fig. 1a–c. The distance traveled along the periphery of the open field was significantly elevated in Dlx1 mutants (mean ± SEM: Dlx1+/+ 1,412 ± 184 cm; Dlx1+/− 1,874 ± 110 cm; Dlx1−/− 2,190 ± 306 cm; F(2,21) = 3.95, p = 0.04) (Fig. 1d). No significant differences were detected among the three groups in center zone entries, time spent in center zone or in periphery, center zone latency, or distance traveled in center zone (data not shown). Also, the percent of time spent in locomotion was significantly enhanced in Dlx1 mutants (mean±SEM: Dlx1+/+ 65.0 ± 7.2%; Dlx1+/− 78.5 ± 2.1 %; Dlx1−/− 86.9 ± 5.3%; F(2,21) = 3.94, p = 0.04) (Fig. 1e). Thus, the Dlx mutants showed locomotor hyperactivity; however, the spatial distribution of this activity did not suggest an anxiety-related phenotype.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Dlx1 mutants displayed locomotor hyperactivity responses in the open field. Representative overlaid traces for the wild-type control (a), Dlx1+/− (b), and Dlx1−/− (c) groups are presented. Dlx1 mutant mice showed significant difference in the mean (± SEM) of distance traveled in periphery of open field (d) and percent of time spent in locomotion (e) in the open field test. Dlx1+/+, n = 9; Dlx1+/−, n = 9; Dlx1−/−, n = 6. * = p < 0.05

Locomotor activity

We monitored locomotor activity levels for three consecutive days. Dlx1−/− mice displayed significantly elevated locomotor responses to novel spatial environments compared to their wild-type littermates (main effect of genotype: F(1,13) = 5.76, p = 0.03; Fig. 2a). Furthermore, total distance traveled was elevated in the Dlx1−/− group during these 3 h (p = 0.02; Fig. 2b). However, the decreasing activity in the locomotor chamber over time also indicated that both wild-type and Dlx1 mutants habituate normally, as shown by the main effect of time (F(1,13) = 35.38, p < 0.0001; Fig. 2a). Moreover, no difference was detected in their rates of habituation to the novel environment, as shown by the lack of interaction between the genotype and the activity over time (F(2,26) = 0.96; p = 0.40, Fig. 2a). This also indicates that Dlx1−/− mice, although habituating normally, consistently showed more movement than their wild-type littermates. No differences were seen in the home cage for activity levels or for their circadian regulation.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Dlx1 mutant mice exhibited elevated locomotor responses to spatial novelty. (a) Both wild-type and Dlx1−/− mice showed normal habituation to the novel environment, but the mutants’ activity was consistently elevated, as shown by the mean (± SEM) of activity count during the first 3 h in the locomotor activity level assay. (b) Locomotion of Dlx1 mutant mice was significantly elevated, especially in the first 3 h of activity, as shown by the mean (± SEM) of distance that the mice traveled. Dlx1+/+, n = 8; Dlx1−/−, n = 7. * = p < 0.05

Food intake

No differences were detected in daily food intake (or body weight) among the three groups of mice (data not shown; Dlx1−/−, n = 7; Dlx1+/−, n = 9; Dlx1+/+, n = 10).

Rotarod

No differences among the three groups were recorded in time on the rotarod, indicating no abnormal motor coordination phenotype in the Dlx1 mutants (data not shown; Dlx1−/−, n = 7; Dlx1+/−, n = 9; Dlx1+/+, n = 10).

Tail suspension

As shown in Fig. 3, the mutant mice exhibited reduced immobility times (mean±SEM: Dlx1+/+ 76.4 ± 17.0 s, Dlx1+/− 34.9 ± 13.7 s, Dlx1−/− 28.6 ± 10.3 s; overall F(2,20) = 3.19, p = 0.06; p = 0.02 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1-/−, p = 0.04 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1+/−). Dlx1−/− mice also showed increased latency to immobility compared to the wild-type controls (mean ± SEM: Dlx1+/+ 117.3 ± 26.5 sec, Dlx1+/− 151.3 ± 21.3 s, Dlx1−/− 223.8 ± 42.7 s; overall F(2,20) = 3.07, p = 0.07; p = 0.03 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1−/−, p = 0.17 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1+/−).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Dlx1 mutants displayed hyperactivity, as indicated by the mean (± SEM) of immobility time (a) and latency to immobility (b) in the tail suspension test. Dlx1+/+, n = 10; Dlx1+/−, n = 6; Dlx1−/−, n = 7. * = p < 0.05

Fear conditioning

After habituation sessions, each mouse in the Dlx1−/−, Dlx1+/−, and Dlx1+/+ groups received a contextual fear conditioning stimulus alone followed 24 h later by a contextual fear test, or combined with a trace fear conditioning session (using a light or tone cue) followed 24 h later by a contextual fear test, followed by a trace fear test after another 24 h. This procedure was performed in four batches of mice consisting of the three genotypes. We did not observe any handling-induced seizures in our fear conditioning assays, including during and after the foot shock.

Baseline freezing

All mice showed similar amounts of baseline freezing in the apparatus: they displayed virtually no freezing before being subjected to the conditioning in Day 1. The baseline freezing for all three groups was also zero, or close to zero, in Day 3 when the mouse was placed in a new context before being subjected to the visual or auditory cue.

Context

Mice were shocked once in the context, and tested for fear response 24 h later. As shown in Fig. 4a, the Dlx1−/− group exhibited reduced freezing compared to the Dlx1+/+ group (mean±SEM: Dlx1+/+ 69.44 ± 6.84%; Dlx1+/− 55.89 ± 8.01%; Dlx1−/− 20 ± 4.32%; overall F(2,23) = 14.08, p = 1.02 × 10−4; p = 8.90 × 10−5 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1−/−, p = 0.10 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1+/−).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Lack of Dlx1 impairs fear conditioning responses in the adult mutant mice. (a) Mean (± SEM) of percent of freezing as a response to the context after fear conditioning (without the light or tone stimulus). In this experiment, one shock after 3-min exposure to the context was administered to elicit the fear response. Baseline freezing before training was zero for all three groups. Dlx1+/+, n = 6; Dlx1+/−, n = 10; Dlx1−/−, n = 10. (b) Mean (± SEM) of percent of freezing as a response to the light cue after trace fear conditioning. In this experiment, the light-pulse-shock procedure was performed three times during the training. Baseline freezing before the light cue (in the novel context) was (or close to) zero for all three groups. Dlx1+/+, n = 6; Dlx1+/−, n = 6; Dlx1−/−, n = 6. (c) Mean (± SEM) of percent of freezing as a response to the tone after trace fear conditioning. One tone-pulse-shock procedure was performed to elicit the fear response. Baseline freezing before the tone (in the novel context) was (or close to) zero for all three groups. Dlx1+/+, n = 6; Dlx1+/, n = 5; Dlx1−/−, n = 10. For all panels, * = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01

We also performed contextual fear conditioning in combination with trace conditioning, and obtained similarly reduced freezing responses in the Dlx1 mutant mice. For all three groups, when mice were shocked three times during the training, freezing responses to the context were higher than if they were shocked only once (Supplemental Fig. 1), with the Dlx1−/− and Dlx1+/− groups showing trends towards reduced freezing compared to the Dlx1+/+ group.

Trace

Trace fear conditioning [26] is a hippocampus-dependent form of associative learning in which the CS and the US are separated by a time interval; when the CS coterminates or overlaps with the US (as in delay fear conditioning), the associative learning does not involve the hippocampus [2729]. In both cases, the amygdala is required [30].

Because homozygous Dlx1 knockout mice (Dlx1−/−) have been reported to have hearing deficits compared to both heterozygous (Dlx1+/−) and wild-type (Dlx1+/+) mice [31], we first assessed fear conditioning with a visual cue. Dlx1−/− mice showed no overt deficits in vision on visual cliff tests and water maze tests (Supplemental Figs. 2 and 3). Following trace fear conditioning, the Dlx1−/− group showed reduced freezing to the light cue relative to the Dlx1+/+ group (Fig. 4b; mean±SEM: Dlx1+/+ 20 ± 4.18%; Dlx1+/− 6.48 ± 2.08%; Dlx1−/− 2.38 ± 1.19%; overall F(2,15) = 3.30, p = 0.06; p = 0.02 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1−/−, p = 0.08 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1+/−).

We obtained similar results in trace fear conditioning using a tone cue (Fig. 4c). Freezing response to the tone was significantly less in Dlx1−/− and Dlx1+/− mice than in Dlx1+/+ mice (mean±SEM: Dlx1+/+ 51.85 ± 4.74%; Dlx1+/− 22.22 ± 4.65%; Dlx1−/− 6.11 ± 2.21%; overall F(2,18) = 11.43, p = 6.25 × 10−4; p = 1.03 × 10−3 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1−/−, p = 0.03 comparing Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1+/−).

Prepulse inhibition

In Dlx1−/− mice (no difference in body weight between wild-type and mutant genotypes in this batch of mice), we found possible deficits in prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle response (Fig. 5). Compared to their wild-type littermates, Dlx1−/− mice showed a trend towards decreased prepulse inhibition at all prepulse intensities that were tested (8 dB, 12 dB, and 16 dB above the background of 60 dB white noise) (mean±SEM: at 8 dB Dlx1+/+ 45.8 ± 5.4%, Dlx1−/− 30.9 ± 3.2%; at 12 dB Dlx1+/+ 47.4 ± 4.9%, Dlx1−/− 38.1 ± 8.7%; at 16 dB Dlx1+/+ 51.2 ± 3.6%, Dlx1−/− 43.1 ± 7.5%; overall ANOVA p < 0.05). A repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of genotype (F(1,69) = 4.07, p < 0.05) and no significant main effect of prepulse intensity (F(2,69) = 0.79, p = 0.46) or interaction between genotype and prepulse intensity (F(2,138) = 0.15, p = 0.86). A post-hoc comparison showed a significant difference between genotypes at 8 dB (p < 0.05), but not at 12 dB or 16 dB above background.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Dlx1−/− mice showed reduced prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response. Analysis was performed at 8, 12, and 16 dB above background. * = p < 0.05. Dlx1+/+, n = 16 (8 females); Dlx1−/−, n = 9 (5 females). Dark bars: Dlx1+/+; light bars: Dlx1

Because this decreased attenuation of the acoustic startle reflex could reflect changes in sensorimotor gating [32] and/or a hearing deficit [31], we analyzed the startle reflex amplitude when no prepulse stimulus was present (Supplemental Fig. 4). The startle response (without a prepulse) at 110 dB was significantly lower for Dlx1−/− mice than Dlx1+/+ mice (mean±SEM in arbitrary units: Dlx1+/+ 920.43 ± 74.90, Dlx1−/− 630.76 ± 105.91; p = 0.04). This finding is consistent with a modest hearing deficit in the Dlx1−/− mice [31].

Spontaneous alternation task (Y-maze)

In light of the deficits in fear conditioning in the Dlx1−/− mice, we also investigated whether these animals had abnormalities in processes such as working memory. The spontaneous alternation task is based on the natural alternation behavior of rodents, and is used to model spatial working memory with a delay time of zero seconds [33]. Dlx1−/− mice performed indistinguishably from their wild-type littermates, and at the same level as C57BL/6 J mice, on this task: the number of entries into maze arms, the percent of alternations ((number of alternations / (total number of arm entries-2)) × 100), and the percent of repeat entries were similar among these three groups for the first 2 min, as well as for the entire 8 min (Supplemental Fig. 5). The results from one-way ANOVA on the above data are summarized in supplemental Table 1.

Performance of a complete alternation on a Y-maze at chance levels has not been found reported in literature. However, if determined mathematically (calculated using probabilities of entries), the chance level of alternation would be 22.22%. All mice tested on this task have performed higher than this level of alternation.

Spatial working memory task (novel arm in a Y-maze)

A two-trial procedure, originally developed to test novelty response and working memory in a Y-maze, was applied to test spatial memory and to determine exploration of novel locations [34]. We used a 2 min interval between trials, to correspond with the limited time span considered to be working memory. This task is known to tax the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) in primates, and medial PFC in rodents [3537]. Proximal visual cues were attached to the arms and to the hub of the maze to orient the mice in space. As indicated by the number of entries into the arms, and the total amount of time spent in each of the arms (Supplemental Fig. 6), Dlx1+/+ and Dlx1−/− mice, as well as C57BL/6 J mice, recognized novel locations, and showed significantly enhanced exploration of novel locations after the 2 min ITI. Thus, the Dlx1−/− mutants behaved normally in this assay.

Using repeated measures ANOVA on the number of entries (Supplemental Fig. 6a), we found that main effect of entries was observed for all groups of mice (F(2,29) = 48.87, p < 0.0001), indicating that all mice explored the novel arm significantly more than the other arms. However, no difference was detected among the mouse groups in the overall level of entries into maze arms (no genotype effect: F(2,29) = 2.44, p = 0.11). Furthermore, all mice showed similar preference for the novel arm entries over the entries to the other arms, as shown by lack of interaction between genotype and number of entries (F(4,58) = 1.98, p = 0.11).

Using repeated measures ANOVA on the time spent in arms (Supplemental Fig. 6b), we found that main effect of time spent in arms was observed for all groups of mice, indicating strong preference for spending time in the novel arm (F(2,29) = 22.56, p < 0.0001). No differences were observed between any of the groups in time spent in maze arms (no genotype effect: F(2,29) = 1.24, p = 0.30). All groups of mice consistently spent more time in the novel arm, as shown by lack of interaction between genotype and time spent in different maze arms (F(4,58) = 0.61, p = 0.66).

In Supplemental Fig. 6c, the novel arm proportions (percent of time spent in novel arm compared to the other two arms) were analyzed using one-sample t-tests comparing the mean of novel arm proportion to the hypothetical 33.33% mean, which would be the "random" proportion for the novel arm. For the percent of novel entries compared to entries into the other two arms, all three groups showed significantly enhanced exploration of the novel arm (C57BL/6 J: p < 0.0001; Dlx1+/+: p = 0.03; Dlx1−/−: p = 0.01). Similar results were found for the percent of time spent in the novel arm compared to in the other two arms (C57BL/6 J: p < 0.0001; Dlx1+/+: p = 0.03; Dlx1−/−: p = 0.13).

Discussion

We found that lack of Dlx1 leads to deficits in fear conditioning and possibly in prepulse inhibition in Dlxl mutant mice. The Dlx1 mutants display hyperactivity in tests of open field, locomotor activity, and tail suspension, but they appear normal in other aspects of general activities and coordination such as food intake, zero maze and rotarod performances. Nonetheless, this phenotype shows specificity, as the mutant mice exhibit normal performance in tasks related to working memory.

Forebrain neural systems mediating fear conditioning

Pavlovian fear conditioning assays are effective at assessing associative learning. Thus, the anomaly in fear learning of the Dlx1 mutants could be secondary to learning deficits. Multiple neural circuits, including those interconnecting the amygdala, hippocampus and PFC, could contribute to their reduction in fear conditioning.

The neural hub for fear conditioning is the amygdala [30]. Sensory stimuli corresponding to the potential CSs from the thalamus, hippocampus, and several cortical regions arrive at the the lateral nucleus of the basolateral complex. The primary output for the generation of conditional fear responses is the central nucleus of the amygdala, which projects to a wide range of regions responsible for emotional responses [30].

The hippocampus is another key component of the fear conditioning system. In contextual fear conditioning, the hippocampus is important for multimodal contextual cues, but is not necessary for conditioning to a discrete unimodal stimulus, such as a tone [3840]. Furthermore, the hippocampus has been implicated in contextual memory retrieval [41]. Muscimol inactivation of the dorsal hippocampus (DH) impairs contextual retrieval of fear memory [42]. Also, inactivation of the ventral hippocampus (VH) impairs memory for context [43]. In trace fear conditioning, the DH is required for normal function [44, 45]. Exposure to a trace-conditioned cue increases immediate early gene expression in the dentate gyrus [46]. Trace conditioning also increases neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus [47], and is impaired by a reduction in newly generated neurons in this region [48].

Finally, the PFC has a critical role in fear conditioning. Trace fear conditioning increases extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) activation in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) immediately following training, and blocking this increase impairs memory tested 48 h later [49]. Studies on trace fear conditioning with intra-mPFC infusion of muscimol indicated that the mPFC activity is required for both recent and remote memory recall [50]. Trace conditioning is associated with increased neuronal activity (c-fos expression) in the anterior cingulate cortex, and is selectively impaired by lesions of this structure. While we did not find evidence indicating working memory-related defects in the PFC of Dlx1 mutants, it is formally possible that abnormal functioning of the mPFC contributed to their fear conditioning phenotype.

Furthermore, PFC function, which is important for spatial working memory tasks, requires continuously keeping spatial information in memory for short (seconds to few minutes) durations. Interneurons in the PFC have a critical role in this process [5153]. Specifically, it has been shown that parvalbumin+ interneurons are important for working memory [54, 55]. The putatively normal parvalbumin-containing interneurons [18] may contribute to the unaltered performance of Dlx1−/− mutants in the working memory tasks described in this report.

Forebrain neural systems mediating behavioral inhibition

Dlx1 mutants also display generalized elevations of activity levels, consistent with deficits in behavioral inhibition. It is possible that such deficits in behavioral inhibition could contribute to reduction of freezing in the fear conditioning assays. Additional studies will be required to determine the relative contributions of behavioral inhibition and associative learning defects to this phenotype. Connections from the PFC to the basal ganglia and cerebellum have been implicated in modulating behavioral inhibition [56, 57]. An impaired ability to inhibit behavior leads to hyperactivity [58], impulsivity [59], perseveration [60], and obsessive/compulsive behavior [59], and is associated with many neuropsychiatric disorders. To understand the anatomical substrates where lack of Dlx1 function contributes to these behaviors, it would be necessary to selectively remove Dlx1 function in the different components of these circuits.

Forebrain neural systems mediating prepulse inhibition

Prepulse inhibition is commonly used as a measure of the sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex [32]. Similar to fear conditioning, prepulse inhibition is regulated by multiple forebrain components, in addition to subcortical regions that process hearing and mediate motor responses (including the cochlea, inferior colliculus and pons). Within the forebrain, the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic circuit is thought to have a central function, with the output through a pathway from the medial prefrontral cortex, accumbens and ventral pallidum to the pons. Inputs to the accumbens from the amygdala, hippocampus and ventral tegmental area also impact this system [32].

Dlx1 is expressed in the above circuits that mediate prepulse inhibition (see below), providing possible explanation for the reduced prepulse inhibition that we observed in the Dlx1 mutants. However, this finding needs to be interpreted with caution. The Dlx genes are expressed in the developing middle ear; Dlx1−/− mutants have dysmorphic ossicles and reduced hearing [31]. These deficits in auditory ability of the mutant mice could result in the small changes in their acoustic startle amplitudes and prepulse inhibition.

Despite the reduced single-stimulus startle reflex amplitude in these mice, the observation that Dlx1 mutant mice do show inhibition of startle responses indicates that they are capable of hearing the prepulse stimulus, and suggests that there may indeed be deficits in prepulse inhibition in these mutant mice. For further investigation of this phenotype, prepulse inhibition using cross-modal stimuli (e.g. light) would be useful.

Dlx1 expression and function in forebrain neural systems

The Dlx genes are probably expressed at some point during development in all forebrain GABAergic neurons, and not in GABAergic neurons in other parts of the central nervous system [16, 6163]. This includes local circuit neurons of the neocortex, hippocampus and amygdala, projection neurons of the basal ganglia (striatum, accumbens, pallidum, septum and central nucleus of the amygdala), and inhibitory neurons of the thalamus (reticular nucleus). Dlx1 is broadly expressed prenatally in these regions. In cortical regions, including the PFC, hippocampus and basolateral and lateral amygdala, Dlx1 is expressed primarily in dendrite-innervating interneurons [18, 64]. In addition, Dlx1 is expressed in the medial amygdala, and perhaps the intercalated nuclei (Wang and Rubenstein, unpublished observations). Thus, Dlx1 could impact many of the key components of the forebrain neural systems that regulate fear conditioning, prepulse inhibition, and behavioral inhibition.

Furthermore, Dlx1 has critical roles in regulating the physiology of the neocortex and hippocampus through promoting inhibitory tone and maintaining interneuron survival [18]. Approximately 30 days after birth, Dlx1−/− mice begin to specifically lose somatostatin+, calretinin+, and NPY+ interneuron subtypes [18], which primarily target dendrites [1, 65]. Around this time, the mutant mice have increased susceptibility to seizures [18].

Currently, there is little information about whether Dlx1−/− mutants have anatomical or physiological defects in other regions of the forebrain where Dlx1 is expressed. While they show reduced numbers of dopaminergic neurons in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus (Yee, Wang, Ekker, Anderson and Rubenstein, submitted), future analyses of additional Dlx1−/− forebrain components could be important in elucidating their abnormal behaviors.

Role of GABAergic signaling in fear conditioning

Similar to the Dlx1−/− mutants, other mouse models with altered inhibitory systems show changes in fear conditioning and general activity. However, unlike the Dlx1−/− mice, many of these show increased indices of fearfulness. For example, GABA A receptor β3 subunit−/− mice have epilepsy, hyperactivity, hypersensitivity to human contact and other sensory stimuli, and lack of coordination [66]. GABA A receptor γ2+/− mice showed enhanced behavioral inhibition toward natural aversive stimuli, heightened responsiveness in trace conditioning and ambiguous cue discrimination learning, and unaffected contextual and delay conditioning [67]. Mice with a point mutation in the α5GABA A receptor gene, which renders the receptor diazepam-insensitive, have selective reduction of α5GABAA receptors only in the hippocampal pyramidal cells and show facilitated trace conditioning but not delay or contextual conditioning [68]. GABA A receptor δ subunit−/− mice have reduced synaptic and extrasynaptic inhibition and decreased sensitivity to neurosteroids [6972], and exhibit enhanced acquisition of tone and context fear [73].

While these GABA receptor mutants generally show increased fear, the Dlx1−/− mutants have reduced fear conditioning. Perhaps this difference is because the receptor mutants affect most types of inhibitory synapses, whereas the Dlx1 mutation appears to selectively affect dendrite-innervating synapses. Thus, future studies are needed to establish the mechanism(s) underlying the reduced fear conditioning of Dlx1−/− mutants, with a particular focus on dissecting the role of Dlx in the PFC, hippocampus and amygdala. Finally, our results raise the possibility that selective defects in subsets of forebrain GABAergic neurons could contribute to human disorders of emotional dysregulation and hyperactivity. This opens the door to considering the possibility that Dlx1 dosage in humans could modulate fear conditioning, as well as the assessment and response to danger.

Altered inhibitory systems have been implicated in multiple brain disorders. Given the critical roles of Dlx1 in interneuron development and function and the deficits in interneurons in Dlx1 mutant mice [18], the behavioral abnormalities in these mutants provide evidence for a link between specific components of the GABAergic system and specific behavioral phenotypes. Thorough investigations of mechanisms underlying these behavioral phenotypes in Dlx1 mutants may provide further understanding on contributions of GABAergic inhibition in complex human neuropsychiatric diseases.

Abbreviations

ITI:

inter-trial interval

CS:

conditioned stimulus

US:

unconditioned stimulus

SPL:

sound pressure level

PFC:

prefrontal cortex

mPFC:

medial prefrontal cortices

DH:

dorsal hippocampus

VH:

ventral hippocampus

References

  1. Markram H, et al. Interneurons of the neocortical inhibitory system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(10):793–807.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Ascoli GA, et al. Petilla terminology: nomenclature of features of GABAergic interneurons of the cerebral cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(7):557–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lewis DA, Hashimoto T, Volk DW. Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(4):312–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Lewis DA, Gonzalez-Burgos G. Neuroplasticity of neocortical circuits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(1):141–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Acosta MT, Pearl PL. The neurobiology of autism: new pieces of the puzzle. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2003;3(2):149–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Rubenstein JL, Merzenich MM. Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2(5):255–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Mohler H, Fritschy JM, Rudolph U. A new benzodiazepine pharmacology. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2002;300(1):2–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Freund TF. Interneuron Diversity series: Rhythm and mood in perisomatic inhibition. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26(9):489–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Brambilla P, et al. GABAergic dysfunction in mood disorders. Mol Psychiatry 2003; 8(8): p. 721–37, 715.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Tunnicliff G, Malatynska E. Central GABAergic systems and depressive illness. Neurochem Res. 2003;28(6):965–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Choudary PV, et al. Altered cortical glutamatergic and GABAergic signal transmission with glial involvement in depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102(43):15653–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Anderson SA, et al. Interneuron migration from basal forebrain to neocortex: dependence on Dlx genes. Science. 1997;278(5337):474–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Anderson SA, et al. Mutations of the homeobox genes Dlx-1 and Dlx-2 disrupt the striatal subventricular zone and differentiation of late born striatal neurons. Neuron. 1997;19(1):27–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Marin O, Anderson SA, Rubenstein JL. Origin and molecular specification of striatal interneurons. J Neurosci. 2000;20(16):6063–76.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Panganiban G, Rubenstein JL. Developmental functions of the Distal-less/Dlx homeobox genes. Development. 2002;129(19):4371–86.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Stuhmer T, et al. Expression from a Dlx gene enhancer marks adult mouse cortical GABAergic neurons. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12(1):75–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Yun K, et al. Modulation of the notch signaling by Mash1 and Dlx1/2 regulates sequential specification and differentiation of progenitor cell types in the subcortical telencephalon. Development. 2002;129(21):5029–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Cobos I, et al. Mice lacking Dlx1 show subtype-specific loss of interneurons, reduced inhibition and epilepsy. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(8):1059–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Crawley JN. Behavioral phenotyping of transgenic and knockout mice: experimental design and evaluation of general health, sensory functions, motor abilities, and specific behavioral tests. Brain Res. 1999;835(1):18–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Crawley JN. Mouse behavioral assays relevant to the symptoms of autism. Brain Pathol. 2007;17(4):448–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Ricceri L, Moles A, Crawley J. Behavioral phenotyping of mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders: relevant social behavior patterns across the life span. Behav Brain Res. 2007;176(1):40–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Qiu M, et al. Role of the Dlx homeobox genes in proximodistal patterning of the branchial arches: mutations of Dlx-1, Dlx-2, and Dlx-1 and -2 alter morphogenesis of proximal skeletal and soft tissue structures derived from the first and second arches. Dev Biol. 1997;185(2):165–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Heisler LK, et al. Elevated anxiety and antidepressant-like responses in serotonin 5-HT1A receptor mutant mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95(25):15049–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Heldt S, et al. Posttraining lesions of the amygdala interfere with fear-potentiated startle to both visual and auditory conditioned stimuli in C57BL/6 J mice. Behav Neurosci. 2000;114(4):749–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Anagnostaras SG, et al. Computer-assisted behavioral assessment of Pavlovian fear conditioning in mice. Learn Mem. 2000;7(1):58–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Pavlov I. Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. London: Oxford UP; 1927.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Ross RT, et al. Hippocampectomy disrupts acquisition and retention of learned conditional responding. Behav Neurosci. 1984;98(2):211–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Paulsen O, Moser EI. A model of hippocampal memory encoding and retrieval: GABAergic control of synaptic plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 1998;21(7):273–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Wallenstein GV, Eichenbaum H, Hasselmo ME. The hippocampus as an associator of discontiguous events. Trends Neurosci. 1998;21(8):317–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Fanselow MS and Poulos AM. The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning. Annu Rev Psychol 2005; 56: p. 207–34.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Polley DB, et al. Severe hearing loss in Dlxl mutant mice. Hear Res. 2006;214(1–2):84–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Geyer MA. Animal models of deficient sensorimotor gating: what we know, what we think we know, and what we hope to know soon. Behav Pharmacol. 2000;11(3–4):185–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Lalonde R. The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26(1):91–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Dellu F, et al. Genetic differences in response to novelty and spatial memory using a two-trial recognition task in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2000;73(1):31–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Kolb B. Functions of the frontal cortex of the rat: a comparative review. Brain Res. 1984;320(1):65–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular and circuit basis of working memory in prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates. Prog Brain Res 1990; 85: p. 325–35; discussion 335–6.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Ongur D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(3):206–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Kim JJ, Fanselow MS. Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science. 1992;256(5057):675–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106(2):274–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Anagnostaras SG, Maren S, Fanselow MS. Temporally graded retrograde amnesia of contextual fear after hippocampal damage in rats: within-subjects examination. J Neurosci. 1999;19(3):1106–14.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Hirsh R. The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: a theory. Behav Biol. 1974;12(4):421–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Holt W, Maren S. Muscimol inactivation of the dorsal hippocampus impairs contextual retrieval of fear memory. J Neurosci. 1999;19(20):9054–62.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Rudy JW, Matus-Amat P. The ventral hippocampus supports a memory representation of context and contextual fear conditioning: implications for a unitary function of the hippocampus. Behav Neurosci. 2005;119(1):154–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. McEchron MD, et al. Hippocampectomy disrupts auditory trace fear conditioning and contextual fear conditioning in the rat. Hippocampus. 1998;8(6):638–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Quinn JJ, et al. Post-training excitotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus attenuate forward trace, backward trace, and delay fear conditioning in a temporally specific manner. Hippocampus. 2002;12(4):495–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Weitemier AZ, Ryabinin AE. Subregion-specific differences in hippocampal activity between Delay and Trace fear conditioning: an immunohistochemical analysis. Brain Res. 2004;995(1):55–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Gould E, et al. Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(3):260–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Shors TJ, et al. Neurogenesis in the adult is involved in the formation of trace memories. Nature. 2001;410(6826):372–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Runyan JD, Moore AN, Dash PK. A role for prefrontal cortex in memory storage for trace fear conditioning. J Neurosci. 2004;24(6):1288–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Blum S, Hebert AE, Dash PK. A role for the prefrontal cortex in recall of recent and remote memories. Neuroreport. 2006;17(3):341–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS. The role of D1-dopamine receptor in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor delayed-response task. J Neurophysiol. 1994;71(2):515–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Rao SG, Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Destruction and creation of spatial tuning by disinhibition: GABA(A) blockade of prefrontal cortical neurons engaged by working memory. J Neurosci. 2000;20(1):485–94.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Sawaguchi T, Iba M. Prefrontal cortical representation of visuospatial working memory in monkeys examined by local inactivation with muscimol. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86(4):2041–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Williams SM, Goldman-Rakic PS, Leranth C. The synaptology of parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1992;320(3):353–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Wang XJ, et al. Division of labor among distinct subtypes of inhibitory neurons in a cortical microcircuit of working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(5):1368–73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Barkley RA. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, self-regulation, and time: toward a more comprehensive theory. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1997;18(4):271–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Brennan AR and Arnsten AF. Neuronal mechanisms underlying attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the influence of arousal on prefrontal cortical function. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1129: p. 236–45.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. 1997;121(1):65–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Chamberlain SR, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20(3):255–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Hauser MD. Perseveration, inhibition and the prefrontal cortex: a new look. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999;9(2):214–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Stuhmer T, et al. Ectopic expression of the Dlx genes induces glutamic acid decarboxylase and Dlx expression. Development. 2002;129(1):245–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Kohwi M, et al. A subpopulation of olfactory bulb GABAergic interneurons is derived from Emx1- and Dlx5/6-expressing progenitors. J Neurosci. 2007;27(26):6878–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Potter GB, et al. Generation of Cre-transgenic mice using Dlx1/Dlx2 enhancers and their characterization in GABAergic interneurons. Mol Cell Neurosci 2008.

  64. Cobos I, et al. Cellular patterns of transcription factor expression in developing cortical interneurons. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16(Suppl 1):i82–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. DeFelipe J. Neocortical neuronal diversity: chemical heterogeneity revealed by colocalization studies of classic neurotransmitters, neuropeptides, calcium-binding proteins, and cell surface molecules. Cereb Cortex. 1993;3(4):273–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Homanics GE, et al. Mice devoid of gamma-aminobutyrate type A receptor beta3 subunit have epilepsy, cleft palate, and hypersensitive behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94(8):4143–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Crestani F, et al. Decreased GABAA-receptor clustering results in enhanced anxiety and a bias for threat cues. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(9):833–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Crestani F, et al. Trace fear conditioning involves hippocampal alpha5 GABA(A) receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99(13):8980–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Mihalek RM, et al. Attenuated sensitivity to neuroactive steroids in gamma-aminobutyrate type A receptor delta subunit knockout mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;96(22):12905–10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Vicini S, Losi G, Homanics GE. GABA(A) receptor delta subunit deletion prevents neurosteroid modulation of inhibitory synaptic currents in cerebellar neurons. Neuropharmacology. 2002;43(4):646–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Porcello DM, et al. Intact synaptic GABAergic inhibition and altered neurosteroid modulation of thalamic relay neurons in mice lacking delta subunit. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89(3):1378–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Spigelman I, et al. Reduced inhibition and sensitivity to neurosteroids in hippocampus of mice lacking the GABA(A) receptor delta subunit. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90(2):903–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Wiltgen BJ, et al. Trace fear conditioning is enhanced in mice lacking the delta subunit of the GABAA receptor. Learn Mem. 2005;12(3):327–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by funds to M.S. (Simons Foundation and the Autism Consortium), R.M. (National Eye Institute Kirschstein-NRSA Fellowship 1 F32 EY017243), D.T.P. (Nancy Lurie Marks Family Foundation), P.H.J. (funds from the state of California), and J.L.R.R. (Nina Ireland and NIMH R37 Grant MH049428). We thank Jed Holtzman and Luna Abdallah for assistance in studies on mouse general activity levels, Li-Huei Tsai, Ji-Song Guan, and Andre Fischer for advice and assistance in fear conditioning assays, Orsolya Kuti for help in maintaining mouse colonies, Yuri Ostrovsky and Cortina McCurry for insightful discussion on data analysis. We are grateful to Sonal Jhaveri for valuable comments on the manuscript.

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Rong Mao or Mriganka Sur.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplemental Table 1

Summary of one way ANOVA results on performances of Dlx1 mutants and wild-type controls in the spontaneous alteration task. (DOC 25 kb)

Supplemental Figure 1

Dlx1 mutants showed trends towards reduced contextual fear conditioning responses after receiving three shocks, as indicated by the mean (±SEM) of percent of freezing as a response to the context (p > 0.05 in all comparisons). The light-pulse-shock procedure was performed three times during the training. Baseline freezing before the training was (or was close to) zero for all three groups. Dlx1+/+, n = 6; Dlx1+/−, n = 6; Dlx1−/−, n = 6.

Supplemental Figure 2

Visual cliff tests on the Dlx1−/− mice did not detect deficits in vision, as shown by the mean (±SEM) of percentage of time spent in the area with high-contrast gratings in normal light (p = 0.34). Similarly, mutant mice did not show significantly different behavior from the control mice in the dark conditions with red light ((p = 0.30). In both conditions, there was a trend for mice to spend more time in the high-contrast area, but this observation is confounded with the experimental procedure that all mice were placed in the high-contrast area at the start of the experiment. Dlx1+/+, n = 5; Dlx1−/−, n = 4.

Supplemental Figure 3

Visual water maze tests did not detect difference in vision between Dlx1−/− mice and their wild-type controls, as indicated by the mean (±SEM) of time spent to find the platform for the groups (p = 0.67). Dlx1+/+, n = 5; Dlx1-/-, n = 4.

Supplemental Figure 4

Dlx1-/- mice show lower startle reflex amplitude than Dlx1+/+ mice in the PPI test. Analysis was performed at a 110 dB startle stimulus (without a prepulse). * = p < 0.05. Dlx1+/+, n = 16 (8 females); Dlx1−/−, n = 9 (5 females).

Supplemental Figure 5

Dlx1 mutants performed similarly to their wild-type controls in the spontaneous alternation task, as indicated by the mean (± SEM) of the number of entries into the maze arms (# Entries), percent of spontaneous alteration (%SA), and percent of repeat entries (% Repeat Entries) for the first 2 min (a) and the entire 8 min (b). The chance level of alteration (detemined using probabilities of entries) is 22.22%. All mice performed higher than the chance level. C57BL/6J, n = 20; Dlx1+/+, n = 6; Dlx1−/−, n = 6. * = p < 0.5.

Supplemental Figure 6

Dlx1 mutants and wild-type controls show no difference in their performance in the spatial working memory task, as illustrated by the mean (±SEM) of the number of entries into the novel arm and other two arms of the Y-maze (a) and total time spent in each of these arms (b) . Asterisks (*) on the novel arm indicate significance levels from ANOVA: all groups of mice showed strong preference for the novel arm. (c) Mean (± SEM) of the percent of novel entries and percent of time spent in the novel arm in this task. The dash line indicates chance level. Asterisks indicate significance levels from t-tests for each genotype. C57BL/6J, n = 20; Dlx1+/+, n = 6; Dlx1−/−, n = 6. For all panels, * = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 2.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mao, R., Page, D.T., Merzlyak, I. et al. Reduced conditioned fear response in mice that lack Dlx1 and show subtype-specific loss of interneurons. J Neurodevelop Disord 1, 224–236 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11689-009-9025-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11689-009-9025-8

Keywords